Autobiographical memory and structural brain changes in chronic phase TBI

Autobiographical memory and structural brain changes in chronic phase TBI

Accepted Manuscript Autobiographical Memory and Structural Brain Changes in Chronic Phase TBI Carrie Esopenko, Brian Levine, Ph.D PII: S0010-9452(17)...

1MB Sizes 6 Downloads 131 Views

Accepted Manuscript Autobiographical Memory and Structural Brain Changes in Chronic Phase TBI Carrie Esopenko, Brian Levine, Ph.D PII:

S0010-9452(17)30011-4

DOI:

10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.007

Reference:

CORTEX 1919

To appear in:

Cortex

Received Date: 18 April 2016 Revised Date:

26 November 2016

Accepted Date: 1 January 2017

Please cite this article as: Esopenko C, Levine B, Autobiographical Memory and Structural Brain Changes in Chronic Phase TBI, CORTEX (2017), doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.007. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

RI PT

Autobiographical Memory and Structural Brain Changes in Chronic Phase TBI

SC

Carrie Esopenkoa1 & Brian Levinea,b*

a. Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, Toronto, Ontario

M AN U

b. University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario

*Corresponding Author: Brian Levine, Ph.D

Baycrest Health Sciences

Toronto, Ontario

AC C

Canada, M6A 2E1

EP

3560 Bathurst Street

TE D

Rotman Research Institute

Email: [email protected] Phone: 416-785-2500 x. 3593; Fax: 416-785-2862

1

Carrie Esopenko is now an Assistant Professor in the Department of Rehabilitation and Movement Sciences, at Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 1 Abstract Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is associated with a range of neuropsychological deficits, including attention, memory, and executive functioning attributable to diffuse axonal injury

RI PT

with accompanying focal frontal and temporal damage. Although the memory deficit of TBI has been well characterized with laboratory tests, comparatively little research has

examined retrograde autobiographical memory at the chronic phase of TBI, with no prior

SC

studies of unselected patients drawn directly from hospital admissions for trauma.

Moreover, little is known about the effects of TBI on canonical episodic and non-episodic

M AN U

(e.g., semantic) autobiographical memory processes. In the present study, we assessed the effects of chronic-phase TBI on autobiographical memory in patients with focal and diffuse axonal injury spanning the range of TBI severity. Patients and socioeconomic- and agematched controls were administered the Autobiographical Interview (Levine, Svoboda,

TE D

Hay, Winocur, & Moscovitch, 2002) a widely used method for dissociating episodic and semantic elements of autobiographical memory, along with tests of neuropsychological and functional outcome. Measures of episodic and non-episodic autobiographical memory were

EP

compared with regional brain volumes derived from high-resolution structural magnetic

AC C

resonance imaging (MRI). Severe TBI (but not mild or moderate TBI) was associated with reduced recall of episodic autobiographical details and increased recall of non-episodic details relative to healthy comparison participants. There were no significant associations between AM performance and neuropsychological or functional outcome measures. Within the full TBI sample, autobiographical episodic memory was associated with reduced volume distributed across temporal, parietal, and prefrontal regions considered to be part

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 2 of the brain’s autobiographical memory network. These results suggest that TBI-related distributed volume loss affects episodic autobiographical recollection. Highlights

EP

TE D

M AN U

SC

RI PT

The Autobiographical Interview was applied in 70 TBI and 22 comparison participants Brain structure was well-characterized with high-resolution structural MRI Severe TBI was associated with reduced episodic and increased non-episodic details Episodic detail reduction was associated with distributed brain volume loss in TBI The diffuse lesion of TBI causes inefficiency in autobiographical memory retrieval

AC C

• • • • •

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 3 1. Introduction Autobiographical memory (AM) tasks draw upon multiple cognitive operations that bring to consciousness details about both personal past episodes (i.e., episodic

RI PT

autobiographical memory) or personal factual information (i.e., personal semantic

memory; Conway, 2001; Levine et al., 2002; Renoult, Davidson, Palombo, Moscovitch, & Levine, 2012). Past neuroimaging work has shown that AM is supported by a network of

SC

regions, including the medial and lateral prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and medial and lateral temporal cortices, and the medial temporal lobes (Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007;

M AN U

Svoboda, McKinnon, & Levine, 2006). Alterations to regions within this network, particularly in the medial temporal lobes, leads to deficits in episodic AM (for review, see Moscovitch, Cabeza, Winocur, & Nadel, 2016; Sheldon, Farb, Palombo, & Levine, 2016; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011). Less is known about the effects of damage to other parts of

TE D

the AM network (for exceptions, see Berryhill, Phuong, Picasso, Cabeza, & Olson, 2007; Bright et al., 2006; Davidson et al., 2008; Kopelman, Stanhope, & Kingsley, 1999; McKinnon et al., 2008).

EP

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is characterized by diffuse axonal injury (DAI;

AC C

Povlishock & Katz, 2005) causing volume loss across the cortical mantle (Levine et al., 2008) as well as focal cortical contusions in ventral frontal and anterior temporal regions (Gentry, Godersky, & Thompson, 1988; Levin, 1989). Given the distributed nature of the functional neuroanatomy of AM, TBI provides a unique lesion model for the understanding brain mechanisms underlying AM. Moreover, it is important for clinical reasons to clarify the effects of TBI on AM as memory complaints in general are a cardinal sign of post-TBI

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 4 cognitive changes (Stuss & Gow, 1992) and the most common cognitive complaint following TBI (Mateer, Sohlberg, & Crinean, 1987). Although TBI is associated with retrograde amnesia and confabulation in the acute

RI PT

phase (for review, see Schacter & Crovitz, 1977), few studies have assessed AM in chronic phase TBI patients. Severe TBI is associated with impaired episodic AM (Carlesimo et al., 1998; Coste et al., 2011; Coste et al., 2015; Knight & O'Hagan, 2009; Levin et al., 1985;

SC

Piolino et al., 2007; Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2014) whereas results in mild to moderate TBI have been mixed, with case study evidence of impairment (e.g., Starkstein, Sabe, & Dorrego,

M AN U

1997) and impairment in university students with concussion history (Barry & Tomes, 2015) but not in veterans with mild blast-related TBI (Palombo et al., 2015). All previous studies of TBI have used small samples recruited post-acute hospitalization, excluding patients with good recovery who did not present for treatment following hospitalization.

TE D

We assessed AM in an unselected sample of 70 patients recruited from initial hospital admission spanning the full spectrum of TBI, along with socioeconomic-matched comparison subjects, using the Autobiographical Interview (AI; Levine et al., 2002). In this

EP

technique, naturalistic autobiographical protocols are transcribed and segmented into

AC C

internal (episodic) or external (non-episodic) details. The AI allows for the parametric quantification of independent measures of episodic and non-episodic memory from within a single narrative, whereas other measures probe these memory processes through separate interviews (e.g., Kopelman, Wilson, & Baddeley, 1989) or focus only on episodic AM (e.g., Piolino, Desgranges, Benali, & Eustache, 2002). The AI assesses memory for events selected by participants prior to testing, isolating elaboration of event details from generation and selection of events that are highly dependent on executive processes.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 5 Finally, we probed five lifetime periods to assess of age-of-memory (i.e., temporal gradient) effects with the prediction that episodic AM impairment in TBI would show a flat temporal gradient, as expected with diffuse damage (Carlesimo et al., 1998; Piolino et al., 2007).

RI PT

The AI has been applied widely in samples of patients with brain disease (e.g., Addis, Moscovitch, & McAndrews, 2007; Irish et al., 2011; McKinnon et al., 2008; Murphy, Troyer, Levine, & Moscovitch, 2008; Rosenbaum et al., 2008), aging (Addis, Wong, & Schacter,

SC

2008; Levine et al., 2002), and psychiatric conditions (McKinnon et al., 2015; Söderlund et al., 2014). It has been applied in one study of nine individuals with moderate-severe TBI

M AN U

(Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2014) and one study of veterans with mild TBI due to blast exposure (Palombo et al., 2015). We predicted that patients with severe TBI would have reduced episodic autobiographical memory relative to comparison participants. Given the conflicting findings in the literature regarding mild-moderate TBI (Barry & Tomes, 2015;

TE D

Palombo et al., 2015), we had no specific prediction for these groups. We also assessed the relationship of AM as assessed by the AI to performance on an extensive battery of neuropsychological tests, including tests of executive function that have been previously

EP

related to AM performance using other measures in TBI (Coste et al., 2011; Coste et al.,

AC C

2015; Piolino et al., 2007).

To our knowledge, there are no published studies assessing the relationship of AM

to structural brain changes in TBI. We assessed the relationship of AM performance to regional brain volumes quantified from high resolution structural MRI scans using a multivariate statistical technique, partial least squares (PLS). We predicted that the recall of internal details would be associated with reduced brain volume in the frontal, temporal, and medial parietal regions known to be associated with AM (Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007;

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 6 Svoboda et al., 2006). Furthermore, given that the AM network is functionally distributed, we predicted that distributed volume loss would be associated with reduced recollection of

2. Materials and Methods 2.1 Participants

RI PT

episodic autobiographical memory.

Patients were recruited from consecutive admissions to Sunnybrook Health

SC

Sciences Centre at approximately one year post-injury as part of the Toronto Traumatic Brain Injury Study (see Fujiwara, Schwartz, Gao, Black, & Levine, 2008; Guild & Levine,

M AN U

2015; Levine et al., 2005; Levine et al., 2008 for neuroimaging, neuropsychological, and behavioral description; Levine et al., 2013). Injury severity was determined by the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), as documented by the trauma team leader’s score at the time of discharge from the Trauma Unit. For most patients, this score corresponded to the

TE D

recommended 6-hour GCS score (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). Severity classification (mild/moderate/severe) was upgraded in 10 cases, where extended loss of consciousness (> 2 hr), post-traumatic amnesia (> 48 hr), or focal lesions suggested more severe injury

EP

than indicated by the GCS. The present study included 70 TBI patients, 15 classified as mild,

AC C

31 classified as moderate, and 24 classified as severe (see Table 1), as well as 22 age-, education, and sex-matched comparison participants. To minimize the effects of socioeconomic factors specific to TBI patients, comparison participants were recruited from friends and family members of the patients. Participants with a history of neurological (aside from the TBI in the patient group) or psychiatric illness or medications known to affect cognitive functioning were excluded. To assess the presence of depression, which can affect AM performance (Söderlund et al., 2014), we examined TBI patients’ scores on

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 7 Component 2 of the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale –Revised (NRS-R; Vanier, Mazaux, Lambert, Dassa, & Levin, 2000; see Guild & Levine, 2015). The mean score was 1.53 (SD = 0.50, range = 2.33), with 91% scoring < 2 (i.e., absent or mild). All participants gave

RI PT

informed, written consent to be involved in the study, which was approved by the local ethics board. 2.2 The Autobiographical Interview

SC

AM was assessed following the Autobiographical Interview procedures as specified in Levine et al., (2002). They will be only briefly described here. Participants were asked to

M AN U

provide a detailed description of a significant personal event from early childhood (to age 11), teenage years (ages 11-17), early adulthood (ages 18-35; 2 events), and the past year. Participants were instructed to recall an event that occurred at a specific time and place. Each memory was assessed across free recall (extemporaneous), general probe

TE D

(clarification of instructions), and specific probe (structured interview to elicit additional contextual details) conditions. We report AM performance analyzed cumulatively across the three levels of cueing.

EP

Transcribed AM protocols were segmented into informational bits or details.

AC C

Details were classified as “internal”, or episodic, if they related directly to the main event described, were specific to time and place, and conveyed a sense of episodic reexperiencing. Internal details were assigned to one of five subcategories (event, place, time, perceptual, and emotion/thought). Details were classified as “external” if they consisted of factual or semantic information, extended to events that did not require recollection of a specific time and place, autobiographical events tangential or unrelated to the main event, repetitions, or other metacognitive statements or editorializing. We report data from the

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 8 main internal and external detail composites. To address significant positive skewness that is typical of AI data, we applied a Winsorization procedure to the data by which scores exceeding +/-2.5 SD from the mean were rescaled to be 2.5 SD from the mean (42 data

McKinnon et al., 2015). 2.3 Neuropsychological and functional outcome test battery

RI PT

points were winsorized, accounting for 3.8% of the scores; McKinnon et al., 2008;

SC

Participants completed a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological and

functional outcome measures described in detail elsewhere (Guild & Levine, 2015; Levine

M AN U

et al., 2013). The neuropsychological battery included the Symbol-Digit Modalities Test (SDMT; Smith, 1978), the Trail-Making Test (Army Individual Test Battery, 1944), the SelfOrdered Pointing Test (SOP; Petrides & Milner, 1982), the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test— Revised (HVLT-R; Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, & Brandt, 1998), Phonemic Word List

TE D

Generation (FAS; Spreen & Strauss, 1998) and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Stuss et al., 2000). In addition to the NRS-R (see above), functional outcome measures included the Sickness Impact Profile (SIP; Bergner, Bobbitt, Carter, & Gilson, 1981), the

EP

Head Injury Family Interview Problem Checklist (HIFI; Kay, Cavallo, Ezrachi, & Vavagiakis,

AC C

1995), the Dysexecutive Function Questionnaire (DEX; Burgess, Alderman, Evans, Emslie, & Wilson, 1998). Both self- and significant other ratings were included for the HIFI and the DEX.

2.4 Image acquisition and processing Sixty of the 70 patients in this sample received MRI scans. Two mild, 5 moderate, and 3 severe TBI patients were not scanned due to MRI contraindications (metal in body, claustrophobia). The procedure for image acquisition and processing is described

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 9 elsewhere (Levine et al., 2008, 2013). Briefly, all participants were scanned with a 1.5 Tesla MRI system at the time of data collection. T1-weighted, T2-weighted, proton density, and gradient echo T2 sequences were obtained. Twenty participants (10 moderate, 10 severe)

RI PT

were identified as having focal cortical contusions largely in frontotemporal areas (8 right, 4 left, and 8 bilateral). These focal lesions, appearing on at least two slices, had a minimal diameter of 3 mm and were manually defined in the axial plane. Further details on the

SC

lesion characteristics can be found in Levine et al., (2008) and Levine et al., (2013).

Brain MRI data were analyzed using a previously reported image processing

M AN U

pipeline (Levine et al., 2008, 2013) for template matching, brain extraction, segmentation of grey matter, white matter, sulcal and ventricular cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), and lesion volumes. A modified Semi-Automated Brain Region Extraction (SABRE) method was used to derive 38 regions of interest (ROIs) customized to fit each patient's brain anatomy.

TE D

Regional CSF, grey matter, and white matter volumes were adjusted for total intracranial capacity using a regression-based method (Arndt, Cohen, Alliger, Swayze, & Andreasen, 1991).

EP

2.5 Statistical analyses

AC C

Behavioral data were assessed via mixed model analysis of variance with planned contrasts coding for Group (i.e., three contrasts of mild, moderate and severe TBI to comparison participants), Time Period (childhood, teenage, early adult, adult, past year, coded as a single linear contrast), and Detail Type (internal, external). An additional ANOVA contrasted patients with lesions to patients without lesions, coding Detail Type and Time as specified above. As the lesion patients all had moderate or severe TBI, patients with mild TBI were excluded from this analysis.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 10 PLS correlation (Krishnan, Williams, McIntosh, & Abdi, 2011), a procedure that characterizes shared variance between two datasets, was used to characterize relationships among the brain and behavior measures. This was accomplished by

RI PT

calculating the correlation between the two covariance matrices, followed by singular value decomposition to identify mutually independent latent variables (LVs) that express the maximal covariance common to both datasets. The reliability of the association was

SC

estimated using split-half resampling with 100 split samples and 1500 permutations

(Kovacevic, Abdi, Beaton, & McIntosh, 2013). The stability of each brain region to the

M AN U

identified LV was determined through bootstrap resampling; subjects were resampled 500 times with 50% of observations resampled with replacement. A variable’s contribution to the LV was considered reliable given a ratio of salience to standard error (hereafter referred to as the bootstrap ratio, interpreted similar to a Z-score) was greater than 2,

TE D

corresponding to 95% confidence limits. As PLS considers all variables in a single computational step, there is no requirement to correct for multiple comparisons. This same analysis was conducted to assess relationships between AI internal and external details

3. Results

AC C

EP

and the neuropsychological and functional outcome tests.

3.1 Behavioral Analysis: Effect of TBI Group and Detail Type To assess the effects of TBI severity on internal vs. external details across the tested

time periods, we examined three-way Group X Detail Type X Time Period interactions for each of the three group contrasts (i.e., mild, moderate, and severe TBI vs comparison participants). As none of these effects involving Time Period were significant, we assessed Group X Detail Type interactions collapsing across Time Period. There was a significant

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 11 Group X Detail Type interaction for the contrast of severe TBI vs. comparison participants, F (1, 88) = 10.06, p = .002, η2= .103. The interactions for the mild and moderate TBI groups as contrasted to the comparison participants were not significant (p > .05). As seen in

RI PT

Figure 1, severe TBI participants recalled fewer internal but more external details relative to comparison participants. Ignoring detail type, there was a significant effect of Time Period on detail generation, F (1, 88) = 20.36, p < .001, η2= .188, corresponding to the

SC

expected forgetting rates (Rubin & Schulkind, 1997). This effect was enhanced in severe TBI vs. comparison participants, F (1, 88) = 4.39, p = .039, η2= .048. These same

M AN U

interactions for the mild and moderate TBI groups vs. comparison participants were not significant. Importantly, there were no significant main effects of Group, suggesting that all three groups generated similar amounts of detail (regardless of detail type). This was supported by an ancillary non-significant one-way ANOVA testing for group effects on total

TE D

details, F(3,88) = 0.687; p = .56). There were no significant main effects or interactions when the lesion and no lesion groups were contrasted. 3.2 Relationship of neuropsychological and functional outcome measures to detail type.

EP

PLS analyses assessing the relationship of AI internal and external details to

AC C

neuropsychological and functional outcome measures yielded no significant LVs. Exploratory analyses of zero-order correlations between internal and external details revealed no significant correlations surviving correction for multiple comparisons. The highest correlations to internal details were noted for the Self Ordered Pointing Test (6, 10 and 12 item version, r’s (68) = .26, .29 and .28, p’s < .05), the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (oral and written, r’s (68) = .26 and .30, p’s < .05), the Trail Making Test (Parts A and B, r’s (68) = .24 and.28, p’s < .05) and Phonemic Word List Generation (FAS, r (68) = .25, p < .05).

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 12 External details were correlated with set loss on the WCST (r (68) = .26, p < .05). Among the functional outcome measures, the only measure to show even a weak relationship to internal details was the HIFI Affective scale (self-rated; r (68) = .25, p < .05). The HIFI

3.3 Relationship of volumetric brain measures to detail type.

RI PT

Physical scale (self rated) was weakly related to external details (r (67) = .24, p < .05).

A single significant LV (p = .004; accounting for 38% of cross-block variance)

SC

identified a pattern of volumetric brain changes across the TBI sample associated with internal, but not external details. Specifically, a lower number of internal details was

M AN U

associated with decreased right medial superior frontal, posterior cingulate gyrus, and bilateral lateral occipital grey matter, as well as left medial orbital frontal and bilateral middle frontal white matter regions. Decreased internal details were also associated with increased CSF volume in the bilateral genual cingulate gyrus, medial temporal lobe,

TE D

anterior basal ganglia, left posterior cingulate gyrus, right anterior cingulate gyrus, and right posterior temporal, superior parietal, inferior parietal, and middle cingulate regions. The split-half permutation analysis indicated that the brain volume pattern and effect of

4. Discussion

AC C

EP

detail type were both reliable (p’s = 0.012 and 0.001, respectively).

AM is a multifaceted cognitive process that is supported by a network of brain

regions including the midline frontoparietal, lateral prefrontal and temporal cortices, as well as the medial temporal lobes (Cabeza & St Jacques, 2007; Svoboda et al., 2006). Alterations to this network, and in particular the medial temporal lobes, lead to deficits in episodic AM (for reviews, see Moscovitch et al., 2016; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011), which occurs in neurodegenerative (Gilboa et al., 2005; Irish et al., 2011; McKinnon et al., 2008)

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 13 and developmental disorders (Addis et al., 2007; Willoughby, McAndrews, & Rovet, 2013), as well as healthy aging (Levine et al., 2002; Piolino et al., 2002). However, it is not well understood how damage outside the medial temporal lobes, as occurs in TBI, affects

RI PT

episodic AM, nor how episodic AM recall is affected by different levels of TBI severity. Nor is it known how AM relates to structural brain alterations in TBI.

We assessed episodic AM in a moderately large sample of patients with TBI using

SC

the AI, which derives separate estimates of episodic and non-episodic processes from within a single autobiographical narrative (Levine et al., 2002). As demonstrated

M AN U

previously (Knight & O'Hagan, 2009; Piolino et al., 2007; Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2014), patients with severe TBI recalled fewer internal, or episodic, details, but more external, non-episodic, details relative to comparison participants across time periods. This effect could not be accounted for by impaired detail generation in general (although severe TBI

TE D

patients did generate fewer details for more remote memories). There was no significant effect of mild or moderate TBI on AM recall. Prior studies of mild TBI have yielded mixed results, with one study showing impaired AM in undergraduates with a history of

EP

concussion (Barry & Tomes, 2015) and another showing no effect on the AI in veterans

AC C

with mild TBI due to blast injuries (Palombo et al., 2015), although the patients’ narratives in that study were less coherent when rated by other measures. AM performance as measured by the AI was neither significantly related to

neuropsychological performance measures nor to functional outcome measures. Considering the zero-order correlations, the neuropsychological tests shared no more than 10% of the variance explained by AI details, explaining why no latent variables relating the two sets of data were revealed by PLS, a sensitive data-driven multivariate analysis

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 14 technique. Previous studies have reported a relationship between measures of executive function and AM performance in TBI (Coste et al., 2015; Piolino et al., 2007; see also Kopelman et al., 2003). The associations were supported by stepwise regression, which

RI PT

yields inflated estimates of association by capitalizing on sampling error specific to the dataset (e.g., see Henderson & Denison, 1989; Judd, McClelland, & Ryan, 2008, pg. 204). Furthermore, the AM measures used, particularly autobiographical fluency (Coste et al.,

SC

2011; Coste et al., 2015), are themselves measures of executive search and generation, whereas in the AI events are generated and selected prior to testing. When specific detail

M AN U

generation was isolated from other generative aspects of event selection in a previous study of AM in TBI, the relationships to executive tasks were attenuated (Coste et al., 2011). At most, liberal interpretation of the zero-order correlations suggests a weak relationship between domain-general tests of executive functioning and internal detail

TE D

generation. Intriguingly, there was an association between set loss on the WCST and external details, also reflecting a lack of task maintenance (i.e., including details not relevant to the central event in the narrative; McKinnon et al., 2015). Yet caution is

EP

warranted in these interpretations, as none of these correlations were significant after

AC C

correction for multiple comparisons. This neuropsychological analysis suggested that AM as assessed by the AI is

independent from laboratory-based tests, particularly measures of executive function (for similar results in frontotemporal dementia, see Irish et al., 2013 and McKinnon et al., 2008). AM tasks that engage domain-general processes, such as fluency tasks, on the other hand, are expected to draw upon executive processes. As noted below, this does not rule

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 15 out the contribution of other prefrontally-mediated processes not assessed by the standardized test battery. The pattern of reduced internal and increased external details in severe TBI relative

RI PT

to comparison subjects mirrors that observed in normal aging (Levine et al., 2002)

corresponding to a general inefficiency in focused AM retrieval. This finding contrasts to previous studies that demonstrated deficits in semantic AM using fluency tasks reliant on

SC

domain-general processes (Coste et al., 2015). The effects in severe TBI could not be

accounted for by presence of focal lesions; they are attributable to diffuse injury. The PLS

M AN U

analyses illustrated the distributed nature of this brain-behavior relationship in TBI, which was consistent with the distributed functional neuroanatomy of AM (Svoboda et al., 2006), such that volume loss over frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital regions corresponded with reduced production of internal details. Bilateral medial temporal and right posterior

TE D

temporal lobe volumes were correlated with internal detail generation, corresponding to the notion of these regions as hubs in a network involving recovery of episodic details (Moscovitch et al., 2016; Sheldon & Levine, 2016; Winocur & Moscovitch, 2011; see also

EP

Bright et al., 2006). The contribution of the right inferior and superior parietal regions is

AC C

consistent with the role of these regions in episodic recollection (Berryhill et al., 2007; Davidson et al., 2008; Rugg & Vilberg, 2013; Simons, Peers, Mazuz, Berryhill, & Olson, 2010).

Lateral prefrontal regions are involved in strategic autobiographical retrieval,

including search and retrieval monitoring operations (Kopelman et al., 1999; Simons & Spiers, 2003). Here, white matter volumes in the middle lateral regions bilaterally contributed to the pattern. Although lateral prefrontal regions are not part of the canonical

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 16 AM network (Svoboda et al., 2006), prefrontal white matter is vulnerable to the effects of TBI (Wilde et al., 2012). Moreover, dorsolateral regions are implicated in monitoring processes when events are more confusable (Cabeza et al., 2004; Stuss et al., 1994), which

RI PT

may have been the case in individuals with more significant TBI. This finding was hinted at by modest correlations between the Self Ordered Pointing Test, a measure of monitoring within working memory, and internal details. Nonetheless, while AI internal details were

SC

not significantly related to behavioral measures of executive functions (see above), these executive measures were also unrelated to prefrontal volumes in this sample (Levine et al.,

M AN U

2013), suggesting that the prefrontally-mediated processes contributing to the AI may not be assessed standardized clinical executive function tasks.

We observed associations between occipital grey matter and internal details, which are strongly influenced by visual processes (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000; Greenberg &

TE D

Knowlton, 2014), with evidence for an association between focal occipital damage and impaired AM (Rubin & Greenberg, 1998; Sheldon et al., 2016). Finally, midline regions including the extent of the cingulate gyrus and inferior and superior medial prefrontal

EP

regions, implicated across the three tissue compartments, are known to be involved in self-

AC C

referential processes, including AM (Buckner & Carroll, 2007). These results provide further evidence that quantified brain volumes are superior to

acute injury characteristics in the assessment of brain-behavior relationships in TBI (Levine et al., 2013). Whereas acute TBI effects on memory (e.g., post-traumatic amnesia, retrograde amnesia) are similar to an amnesic state, the present results indicate that at the chronic stage, a distributed lesion affecting multiple aspects of the AM network causes a generalized inefficiency of retrieval (e.g., recovery of details, reconstruction, elaboration,

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 17 and monitoring) that is distinct from that observed in medial temporal lobe amnesia (Rosenbaum et al., 2008). Although episodic AM was only impaired in the patients classified as severe, the

RI PT

observed brain-behavior relationships held across the full sample of TBI patients,

indicating volumetric changes across the TBI severity spectrum can affect episodic AM even if they are only detectable at the group level in patients with severe TBI. When

SC

patients with chronic-phase TBI complain of memory changes, they are referring to

everyday memory such as AM, not lists of words of pictures. The distributed anatomy

M AN U

supporting AM is distinct from that supporting laboratory tasks (McDermott, Szpunar, & Christ, 2009). The present findings suggest that a complete understanding of the functional

AC C

EP

TE D

mnemonic deficits in chronic TBI require assessment of AM.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 18 Figure Captions Figure 1. Internal and external details recalled in mild, moderate, and severe TBI groups and comparison subjects. Details are averaged across time periods. A significant interaction

RI PT

between Detail Type (internal/external) and Group was noted for a planned contrast of the severe group vs. comparison participants, indicating that the severe TBI patients produced fewer internal and more external details than comparison participants. There were no

SC

significant effects of group for the mild and moderate TBI patients. Error bars represent standard error. Bars: white = comparison participants; dots = mild TBI; lines = moderate

M AN U

TBI; black = severe TBI.

Figure 2. Latent variable from PLS analysis indicating the association of internal details with patterns of volume changes across CSF, grey matter, and white matter in TBI patients. Panels A, B, and C reflect brain regions significantly correlated with internal details (r=

TE D

0.19). The color bar indicates the coding scheme according to the level of the bootstrap ratio, interpreted as a Z-score. CSF values are negative (Panel A), indicating volume increases associated with fewer internal details, whereas grey and white matter values are

EP

positive (Panels B and C, respectively), indicating volume reductions associated with fewer

AC C

internal details. Images were thresholded at a bootstrap ratio of 2.0, corresponding approximately to p < .05. Axial images are displayed in radiological convention (right hemisphere displayed on left side of image). The right and left cingulate volumes are displayed on the right and left side of the images, respectively.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 19 Acknowledgements The TBI and comparison participants are thanked for volunteering their time and effort to this research. Ann Campbell, Catherine Hynes, Sabitha Kanagasabai, Charlene

RI PT

O’Connor, Colleen O’Toole, Marina Mandic, Karen Philp, Adriana Fecko, Jovanka Skocic, and Gary Turner are thanked for technical assistance. Natasa Kovacevic, Fuqiang Gao, Joel

Ramirez, and Sandra Black are thanked for assistance with the neuroimaging pipeline. We

SC

gratefully thank the TBI participants and non-injured volunteers for participating in this research. This research was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health

M AN U

Research (Grant #sMT-14744, MOP-37535, and MOP-108540), and the NIH-NICHD (Grant #HD42385-01) to B.L., and an Alzheimer's Society of Canada’s Research Program Post

AC C

EP

TE D

Doctoral Fellowship awarded to C.E.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 20 References Addis, D. R., Moscovitch, M., & McAndrews, M. P. (2007). Consequences of hippocampal damage across the autobiographical memory network in left temporal lobe epilepsy.

RI PT

Brain : a journal of neurology, 130(Pt 9), 2327-2342. doi:10.1093/brain/awm166 Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Age-related changes in the episodic

9280.2008.02043.x

SC

simulation of future events. Psychological science, 19(1), 33-41. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

Army Individual Test Battery. (1944). Manual of Directions and Scoring. Washington, D.C.:

M AN U

War Department, Adjutant General's Office.

Arndt, S., Cohen, G., Alliger, R. J., Swayze, V. W., 2nd, & Andreasen, N. C. (1991). Problems with ratio and proportion measures of imaged cerebral structures. Psychiatry research, 40(1), 79-89. doi:10.1016/0925-4927(91)90031-K

TE D

Barry, N. C., & Tomes, J. L. (2015). Remembering your past: The effects of concussion on autobiographical memory recall. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 37(9), 994-1003. doi:10.1080/13803395.2015.1038981

EP

Benedict, R. H. B., Schretlen, D., Groninger, L., & Brandt, J. (1998). Hopkins verbal learning

AC C

test— revised: normative data and analysis of inter-form and test– retest reliability. The Clinical neuropsychologist, 12(1), 43– 55. doi:10.1076/clin.12.1.43.1726

Bergner, M., Bobbitt, R. A., Carter, W. B., & Gilson, B. S. (1981). The Sickness Impact Profile: development and final revision of a health status measure. Med Care, 19(8), 787805. doi:10.1097/00005650-198108000-00001 Berryhill, M. E., Phuong, L., Picasso, L., Cabeza, R., & Olson, I. R. (2007). Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 21 memory. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27(52), 14415-14423. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-07.2007 Bright, P., Buckman, J., Fradera, A., Yoshimasu, H., Colchester, A. C., & Kopelman, M. D.

RI PT

(2006). Retrograde amnesia in patients with hippocampal, medial temporal, temporal lobe, or frontal pathology. Learning & memory, 13(5), 545-557. doi:10.1101/lm.265906

SC

Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(2), 49-57. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004

M AN U

Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 4(6), 547-558.doi:10.1017/S1355617798466037 Cabeza, R., Prince, S. E., Daselaar, S. M., Greenberg, D. L., Budde, M., Dolcos, F., . . . Rubin, D. C.

TE D

(2004). Brain activity during episodic retrieval of autobiographical and laboratory events: an fMRI study using a novel photo paradigm. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1583-1594. doi:10.1162/0898929042568578

EP

Cabeza, R., & St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional neuroimaging of autobiographical memory.

AC C

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219-227. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.005 Carlesimo, G. A., Sabbadini, M., Bombardi, P., Di Porto, E., Loasses, A., & Caltagirone, C. (1998). Retrograde memory deficits in severe closed-head injury patients. Cortex; a

journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 34(1), 1-23. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70734-2

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 22 Conway, M. A. (2001). Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: autobiographical memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 356(1413), 1375-1384. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0940

RI PT

Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical

memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261-288. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.107.2.261

SC

Coste, C., Agar, N., Petitfour, E., Quinette, P., Guillery-Girard, B., Azouvi, P., & Piolino, P. (2011). Exploring the roles of the executive and short-term feature-binding

M AN U

functions in retrieval of retrograde autobiographical memories in severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 47(7), 771-786. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.004 Coste, C., Navarro, B., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Brami, M., Azouvi, P., & Piolino, P. (2015). Disruption

TE D

of temporally extended self-memory system following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 71, 133-145. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.014 Davidson, P. S., Anaki, D., Ciaramelli, E., Cohn, M., Kim, A. S., Murphy, K. J., . . . Levine, B.

EP

(2008). Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory? Evidence from focal

AC C

lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1743-1755. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.011

Fujiwara, E., Schwartz, M. L., Gao, F., Black, S. E., & Levine, B. (2008). Ventral frontal cortex functions and quantified MRI in traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 46(2),

461-474. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.027

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 23 Gentry, L. R., Godersky, J. C., & Thompson, B. (1988). MR imaging of head trauma: review of the distribution and radiopathologic features of traumatic lesions. American Journal of Roentgenology, 150(3), 663-672. doi:10.2214/ajr.150.3.663

RI PT

Gilboa, A., Ramirez, J., Kohler, S., Westmacott, R., Black, S. E., & Moscovitch, M. (2005).

Retrieval of autobiographical memory in Alzheimer's disease: relation to volumes of medial temporal lobe and other structures. Hippocampus, 15(4), 535-550.

SC

doi:10.1002/hipo.20090

Greenberg, D. L., & Knowlton, B. J. (2014). The role of visual imagery in autobiographical

M AN U

memory. Memory & Cognition, 42(6), 922-934. doi:10.3758/s13421-014-0402-5 Guild, E. B., & Levine, B. (2015). Functional correlates of midline brain volume loss in chronic traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 21(8), 650-655. doi:10.1017/S1355617715000600

TE D

Henderson, D. A., & Denison, D. R. (1989). Stepwise Regression in Social and Psychological Research. Psychological Reports, 64(1), 251-257. doi:10.2466/pr0.1989.64.1.251 Irish, M., Hodges, J. R., & Piguet, O. (2013). Episodic future thinking is impaired in the

EP

behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Cortex, 49(9), 2377-2388. doi:

AC C

10.1016/j.cortex.2013.03.002 Irish, M., Hornberger, M., Lah, S., Miller, L., Pengas, G., Nestor, P. J., Hodges, J.R., Piguet, O. (2011). Profiles of recent autobiographical memory retrieval in semantic dementia, behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia, and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2694-2702. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.017 Judd, C. M., McClelland, G. G., & Ryan, C. R. (2008). Data Analysis: A Model Comaprison Approach (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. doi:10.1198/016214507000001012

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 24 Kay, T., Cavallo, M. M., Ezrachi, O., & Vavagiakis, P. (1995). The Head Injury Family Interview: A clinical research tool. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 10(2), 1231.

RI PT

Knight, R. G., & O'Hagan, K. (2009). Autobiographical memory in long-term survivors of severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(5), 575-583. doi:10.1080/13803390802363710

SC

Kopelman, M. D., Lasserson, D., Kingsley, D. R., Bello, F., Rush, C., Stanhope, N., . . .

Colchester, A. C. (2003). Retrograde amnesia and the volume of critical brain

M AN U

structures. Hippocampus, 13(8), 879-891. doi:10.1002/hipo.10140 Kopelman, M. D., Stanhope, N., & Kingsley, D. (1999). Retrograde amnesia in patients with diencephalic, temporal lobe or frontal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 37(8), 939-958. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00143-2

TE D

Kopelman, M. D., Wilson, B. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). The autobiographical memory interview: a new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11(5), 724-

EP

744. doi:10.1080/01688638908400928

AC C

Kovacevic, N., Abdi, H., Beaton, D., & McIntosh, A. R. (2013). Revisiting PLS Resampling: Comparing Significance vs. Reliability Across Range of Simulations. In H. Abdi, Chin,

W., Vinzi, E.V., Russolillo, G., & Trinchera, L. (Ed.), New Perspectives in Partial Least Squares and Related Methods. New York: Springer Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-1-46148283-3

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 25 Krishnan, A., Williams, L. J., McIntosh, A. R., & Abdi, H. (2011). Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage, 56(2), 455-475. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034

RI PT

Levin, H. S. (1989). Memory deficit after closed head injury. In F. G. Boller, J. (Ed.),

Handbook of Neuropsychology (Vol. 3, pp. 183-207). New York: Elsvier Science Publishers. doi:10.1080/01688639008400960

SC

Levin, H. S., High, W. M., Meyers, C. A., Von Laufen, A., Hayden, M. E., & Eisenberg, H. M. (1985). Impairment of remote memory after closed head injury. Journal of

M AN U

Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 48(6), 556-563. doi:10.1136/jnnp.48.6.556 Levine, B., Black S.E., Cheung, G., Campbell, A., O'Toole, C., & Schwartz, M. L. (2005). Gambling task performance in traumatic brain injury: relationships to injury severity, atrophy, lesion location, and cognitive and psychosocial outcome. Cognitive

TE D

and Behavioral Neurology, 18, 45-54. doi:10.1097/01.wnn.0000152227.13001.c3 Levine, B., Kovacevic, N., Nica, E. I., Cheung, G., Gao, F., Schwartz, M. L., & Black, S. E. (2008). The Toronto traumatic brain injury study: injury severity and quantified MRI.

EP

Neurology, 70(10), 771-778. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000304108.32283.aa

AC C

Levine, B., Kovacevic, N., Nica, E. I., Schwartz, M. L., Gao, F., & Black, S. E. (2013). Quantified MRI and cognition in TBI with diffuse and focal damage. NeuroImage. Clinical, 10(2), 534-541. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.015

Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677-689.doi:10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.677

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 26 Mateer, C. A., Sohlberg, M. M., & Crinean, J. (1987). Focus on clinical research: Perceptions of memory function in individuals with closed-head injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 2(3), 74-84.doi: 10.1097/00001199-198709000-00009

RI PT

McDermott, K. B., Szpunar, K. K., & Christ, S. E. (2009). Laboratory-based and

autobiographical retrieval tasks differ substantially in their neural substrates.

Neuropsychologia, 47(11), 2290-2298. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.025

SC

McKinnon, M. C., Nica, E. I., Sengdy, P., Kovacevic, N., Moscovitch, M., Freedman, M., . . . Levine, B. (2008). Autobiographical memory and patterns of brain atrophy in

M AN U

frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(10), 18391853. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20126

McKinnon, M. C., Palombo, D. J., Nazarov, A., Kumar, N., Khuu, W., & Levine, B. (2015). Threat of death and autobiographical memory: a study of passengers from Flight

TE D

AT236. Clinical Psychological Science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3(4), 487-502. doi:10.1177/2167702614542280 Moscovitch, M., Cabeza, R., Winocur, G., & Nadel, L. (2016). Episodic Memory and Beyond:

EP

The Hippocampus and Neocortex in Transformation. Annual Review of Psychology,

AC C

67, 105-134. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143733 Murphy, K. J., Troyer, A. K., Levine, B., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Episodic, but not semantic, autobiographical memory is reduced in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 46(13), 3116-3123. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.004

Palombo, D. J., Kapson, H. S., Lafleche, G., Vasterling, J. J., Marx, B. P., Franz, M., & Verfaellie, M. (2015). Alterations in autobiographical memory for a blast event in Operation

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 27 Enduring Freedom and Operation Iraqi Freedom veterans with mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 29(4), 543-549. doi:10.1037/neu0000198 Petrides, M., & Milner, B. (1982). Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and

RI PT

temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 20(3), 249-262. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Benali, K., & Eustache, F. (2002). Episodic and semantic remote

doi:10.1080/09658210143000353

SC

autobiographical memory in ageing. Memory, 10(4), 239-257.

M AN U

Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Manning, L., North, P., Jokic, C., & Eustache, F. (2007). Autobiographical memory, the sense of recollection and executive functions after severe traumatic brain injury. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior, 43(2), 176-195. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70474-X

TE D

Povlishock, J. T., & Katz, D. I. (2005). Update of neuropathology and neurological recovery after traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 20(1), 7694. doi:10.1097/00001199-200501000-00008

EP

Rasmussen, K. W., & Berntsen, D. (2014). Autobiographical memory and episodic future

AC C

thinking after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neuropsychology, 8(1), 34-52. doi:10.1111/jnp.12003

Renoult, L., Davidson, P. S., Palombo, D. J., Moscovitch, M., & Levine, B. (2012). Personal semantics: at the crossroads of semantic and episodic memory. Trends in Cognitive

Sciences, 16(11), 550-558. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.003 Rosenbaum, R. S., Moscovitch, M., Foster, J. K., Schnyer, D. M., Gao, F., Kovacevic, N., . . . Levine, B. (2008). Patterns of autobiographical memory loss in medial-temporal

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 28 lobe amnesic patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(8), 1490-1506. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20105 Rubin, D. C., & Greenberg, D. L. (1998). Visual memory-deficit amnesia: a distinct amnesic

RI PT

presentation and etiology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(9), 5413-5416.doi:10.1073/pnas.95.9.5413

Rubin, D. C., & Schulkind, M. D. (1997). The distribution of autobiographical memories

SC

across the lifespan. Memory & Cognition, 25(6), 859-866.doi:10.3758/BF03211330 Rugg, M. D., & Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval.

M AN U

Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(2), 255-260. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005 Schacter, D. L., & Crovitz, H. F. (1977). Memory function after closed head injury: a review of the quantitative research. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous

9452(77)80006-3

TE D

system and behavior, 13(2), 150-176. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-

Sheldon, S., Farb, N., Palombo, D. J., & Levine, B. (2016). Intrinsic medial temporal lobe connectivity relates to individual differences in episodic autobiographical

EP

remembering. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and

AC C

behavior, 74, 206-216. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.005 Sheldon, S., & Levine, B. (2016). The role of the hippocampus in memory and mental construction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. doi:10.1111/nyas.13006

Simons, J. S., Peers, P. V., Mazuz, Y. S., Berryhill, M. E., & Olson, I. R. (2010). Dissociation between memory accuracy and memory confidence following bilateral parietal lesions. Cerebral Cortex, 20(2), 479-485. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp116

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 29 Simons, J. S., & Spiers, H. J. (2003). Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in longterm memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 4(8), 637-648. doi:10.1038/nrn1178 Smith, A. (1978). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angelos: Western Psychological Services.

RI PT

Söderlund, H., Moscovitch, M., Kumar, N., Daskalakis, Z. J., Flint, A., Herrmann, N., & Levine, B. (2014). Autobiographical episodic memory in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(1), 51-60. doi:10.1037/a0035610

SC

Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press.

M AN U

Starkstein, E., Sabe, L., & Dorrego, M. F. (1997). Severe Retrograde Amnesia After a Mild Closed Head Injury. Neurocase, 3, 105-109. doi:10.1080/13554799708404043 Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Palumbo, C. L., Buckle, L., Sayer, L., & Pogue, J. (1994). Organizational strategies with unilateral or bilateral frontal lobe injury in word

TE D

learning tasks. Neuropsychology, 8(3), 355-373. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.3.355 Stuss, D. T., & Gow, C. A. (1992). "Frontal dysfunction" after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 5(4), 272-282.

EP

Stuss, D. T., Levine, B., Alexander, M. P., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Hamer, L., . . . Izukawa, D.

AC C

(2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 38(4), 388-402.doi:10.1016/S0028-

3932(99)00093-7

Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., & Levine, B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2189-2208. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Autobiographical memory in TBI - 30 Teasdale, G., & Jennett, B. (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet, 2, 81-84. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0 Vanier, M., Mazaux, J. M., Lambert, J., Dassa, C., & Levin, H. S. (2000). Assessment of

RI PT

neuropsychologic impairments after head injury: interrater reliability and factorial and criterion validity of the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale-Revised. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 81(6), 796-806.doi:10.1016/S0003-

SC

9993(00)90114-X

Wilde, E. A., Ayoub, K. W., Bigler, E. D., Chu, Z. D., Hunter, J. V., Wu, T. C., . . . Levin, H. S.

M AN U

(2012). Diffusion tensor imaging in moderate-to-severe pediatric traumatic brain injury: changes within an 18 month post-injury interval. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 6(3), 404-416. doi:10.1007/s11682-012-9150-y Willoughby, K. A., McAndrews, M. P., & Rovet, J. (2013). Effects of early thyroid hormone

TE D

deficiency on children's autobiographical memory performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 19(4), 419-429. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001488

EP

Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2011). Memory transformation and systems consolidation.

AC C

Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 17(5), 766-780. doi:10.1017/S1355617711000683

Zachary, R. A. (1986). Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Revised Manual. Los Angelos: Western Psychological Services.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Table 1: Characteristics of all TBI patients and comparison participants

SD 2.6 2.8 2.4 2.7 1.9

SD 6.0 5.9 6.4 5.7 -

GCS3 Mean 10.1 14.6 11.1 5.9 -

SD 3.9 0.7 2.2 2.5 -

LOC4 Median 48.0 0.0 18.0 144.0 -

IQR 167 0 149 276 -

RI PT

Vocabulary2 Mean 27.8 27.9 28.4 27.0 -

SC

70 (43) 15 (9) 31 (17) 24 (17) 22 (10)

SD 10.4 12.5 10.8 7.9 8.3

Education Mean 14.4 13.7 14.8 14.2 15.0

EP

TE D

Note: 1. Number of males in parentheses. 2. Raw score on the vocabulary subtest of the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (Zachary, 1986). 3. Glasgow Coma Scale score (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). 4. Duration of loss of consciousness in hours 5. Duration of post-traumatic amnesia in hours 6. Time since injury in years

AC C

TOTAL Mild Moderate Severe Controls

Age Mean 31.0 32.5 32.5 28.2 27.9

M AN U

N1

PTA5 Median 336 0 324 1008 -

IQR 810 2 354 936 -

TSI6 Mean 1.1 1.2 1.1 1.0 -

SD 0.4 0.5 0.4 0.2 -

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Figure 1.

70

*

RI PT

60

SC M AN U

40

TE D

30

*

EP

20

10

AC C

Number of Details

50

0 Internal

External Detail Type

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT Figure 2.

SC

RI PT

CSF

Panel A:

White Matter

AC C

Panel C:

EP

TE D

Grey Matter

M AN U

Panel B:

3

2

1

0

-1

-2

-3