Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Physiology & Behavior j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / p h b
Brain distribution of genes related to changes in locomotor activity Pasquale Mignogna, Davide Viggiano ⁎ Department of Health Sciences, University of Molise, Campobasso, 86100, Italy
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history: Received 2 August 2009 Received in revised form 19 November 2009 Accepted 26 January 2010 Keywords: Knockout Mice Locomotor activity Exploration mRNA expression
a b s t r a c t The relationship between genes and behavior, and particularly the hyperactive behavior, is clearly not linear nor monotonic. To address this problem, a database of the locomotor behavior obtained from thousands of mutant mice has been previously retrieved from the literature. Data showed that the percent of genes in the genome related to locomotor hyperactivity is probably more than 1.56%. These genes do not belong to a single neurotransmitter system or biochemical pathway. Indeed, they are probably required for the correct development of a specific neuronal network necessary to decrease locomotor activity. The present paper analyzes the brain expression pattern of the genes whose deletion is accompanied by changes in locomotor behavior. Using literature data concerning knockout mice, 46 genes whose deletion was accompanied by increased locomotor behavior, 24 genes related to decreased locomotor behavior and 23 genes not involved in locomotor behavior (but important for other brain functions) have been identified. These three groups of genes belonged to overlapping neurotransmitter systems or cellular functions. Therefore, we postulated that a better predictor of the locomotor behavior resulting from gene deletion might be the brain expression pattern. To this aim we correlated the brain expression of the genes and the locomotor activity resulting from the deletion of the same genes, using two databases (Allen Brain Atlas and SymAtlas). The results showed that the deletion of genes with higher expression level in the brain had higher probability to be accompanied by increased behavioral activity. Moreover the genes that were accompanied by locomotor hyperactivity when deleted, were more expressed in the cerebral cortex, amygdala and hippocampus compared to the genes unrelated to locomotor activity. Therefore, the prediction of the behavioral effect of a gene should take into consideration its brain distribution. Moreover, data confirmed that genes highly expressed in the brain are more likely to induce hyperactivity when deleted. Finally, it is suggested that gene mutations linked to specific behavioral abnormalities (e.g. inattention) might probably be associated to hyperactivity if the same gene has elevated brain expression. © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction The current tendency of biological psychiatry to focus on the genetic substrates of mental health and disease has driven much research to find gene mutations in different mental diseases, hoping to predict the risk to develop a mental illness or to find new pharmacological targets [1,2] (see the work by C. Lombroso in the last century for a more historical perspective about this approach [3,4]).
Abbreviations: CB, cerebellum; CTX, cerebral cortex; HIP, hippocampus; HPF, hippocampal formation; HY, hypothalamus; LSX, lateral septal complex; MB, midbrain; MY, medulla; OLF, olfactory regions; P, Pons; PAL, Pallidum; RHP, retrohippocampal region; STR, striatum; TH, thalamus; AMY, striatum-like amygdala nuclei; SN, substantia nigra; FC, frontal cortex; MOB, medial olfactory epithelium; SC, spinal cord. ⁎ Corresponding author. Dept. Health Sciences, Univ. of Molise, Via De Sanctis III Edificio Polifunzionale, 86100 Campobasso, Italy. Tel.: +39 0874 404965; fax: +39 0874 404763. E-mail address:
[email protected] (D. Viggiano). 0031-9384/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.026
The recent technology of transgenic mice allowed to apply this paradigm through the study of the behavioral effects of deletions, insertions or mutations of single genes in mice. The main conclusion drawn from the large amount of data now available is that the relationship between behavioral tendency and gene mutation/ deletion is not linear nor monotonic, because the effects of multiple genetic changes may be completely different from the sum of the effects induced by individual genes, and therefore the results of multiple genetic modifications are often unpredictable [5,6]. Indeed, the number of transgenic mice produced up to now is very large: an Internet source lists at least 5283 different transgenic animals including animals with genes deletions (knockout mice; see http://www.informatics.jax.org/imsr/IMSRSearchForm.jsp). Interestingly, most of these animals have been behaviorally tested, but a database describing/comparing the behavioral pattern of each transgenic mouse is not yet available (however, see ref. [7] and the database of references describing individual knockout mice available at http://www.bioscience.org/knockout/knochome.htm). Such a database would allow to study the relationship between genes and behavior, and the identification of the animals that show a specific behavioral deficit
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
within the entire set of transgenic models (metanalytical approach; see also Mouse Genome Informatics (MGI) projects [8] and the web site http://www.informatics.jax.org/phenotypes.shtml). The comparison of behavioral features of transgenic animals reported in different scientific articles should consider several problems, such as the differences in behavioral testing procedures (see e.g. [9]), the age and sex of the animals (see e.g. ref. [10,11]), sample size, rearing conditions such as number of animals per cage (see e.g. [12]) and the genetic background. All these parameters may affect the results and the reproducibility of data among laboratories [5]. A previous report classifying the behavioral activity of mutant mice based on numerous articles about transgenic mice, concluded that the number of genes linked to the increase of locomotor activity might be very high [13]. Specifically, data pertaining all transgenic mice with increased or decreased locomotor activity were retrieved and filtered to limit the effect of confounding variables. Unexpectedly, the results identified a very large number of genes involved in hyperactivity after exposure to a novel environment, which have been estimated to be at least 1.56% of the genes in the genome [13]. It is unclear why the mutation of so many genes can increase locomotor activity. Moreover, it was not possible to group all these genes in just one or few neurotransmitter systems or biochemical pathways [13]. Therefore, a working hypothesis suggested that all these genes were required for the correct development/functioning of a common neuronal network necessary to decrease a spontaneously occurring hyperactivity [13]. The present article tests this hypothesis through the analysis of brain expression of genes related to changes (increase/ decrease) in locomotor behavior. To this aim a database of knockout mice tested for locomotor activity in a novel environment has been realized from published studies, filtered on the basis of inclusion/ exclusion criteria. The knockout mice have been divided in hyperactive, hypoactive and normoactive groups on the basis of their locomotor behavior. Finally, the basal brain distribution of the genes knocked out in these groups using two databases available in Internet (Allen Brain Atlas and SymAtlas) has been studied.
2. Materials and methods The complete list of genes described in this paper is reported in Table 1. This list has been derived from an initial set of 15,806 abstracts containing key words “locomotor”, “activity”, “mice”, retrieved from PubMed (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/sites/entrez/). This set was then filtered to include only the publications describing knockout mice and their locomotor behavior. These publications were then organized in a table containing: (i) the name of the gene knocked out, (ii) various parameters about the behavioral test, such as test duration, number of repetitions, size and shape of the arena, behavioral procedures, horizontal and vertical (rearing) activity, measurement system (photocells, video tracking etc), (iii) data about the sample, such as sample size, age and sex of the animals, type of control mice, genetic background, and (iv) statistical analysis (effect size expressed as % change compared to wt littermates and significance level). Finally, the animals in this first table have been selected according to the following inclusion criteria: (1) Age of animals from 2 to 6 months; (2) Minimum 7 animals per experimental group, each individually tested; (3) Experimental groups consisting of only males (no females); (4) Behavioral observations about horizontal locomotor activity (changes in rearing frequency were not considered); (5) Control mice consisting of wild type littermates; (6) Genes had to be expressed in the brain (this information has been retrieved from the Allen Brain Atlas and SymAtlas, as described below).
619
The following exclusion criteria have been used: (1) The behavioral data did not discriminate male from female mice as these were grouped together; (2) Animals were not studied in a novel environment; (3) Behavioral changes occurred only for very short intervals (less than 5 min) or after very long exposure to novelty (more than 30 min) or only after repeated exposures to novelty; (4) Mice were prepuberal or aged; (5) Inappropriate control groups (no wt-littermates) or small experimental groups with less than seven individuals; (6) Genes were not expressed in the brain (this exclusion criterion was used to minimize the possibility that the alterations in locomotor behavior were due to systemic effects of the genetic deletion). The investigations that fulfilled all these requirements were then used to categorize the locomotor activity. The animals were divided in three groups, according their locomotor activity: (i) Increased activity or (ii) decreased activity, if locomotor activity changed with an effect size (increase or decrease respectively) greater than 20%; and (iii) normoactive animals, showing an effect size smaller than 10% compared to their wt littermate controls, which was not statistically significant (therefore animals showing no significant differences from controls but with an effect size greater than 10% were excluded too). Animals in the normoactive group satisfied the following additional criteria: 1) the targeted gene had no known “isoforms” which could replace its function, and 2) the targeted gene was involved in brain functions, since neurological/behavioral abnormalities (e.g. anxiety, memory deficit etc) were also reported. Afterwards, the brain expression pattern of the candidate genes has been retrieved from two different databases: the Allen Brain Atlas (http://mouse.brain-map.org), a database describing the mRNA brain expression pattern of mouse genes based on the in situ hybridization technique and the SymAtlas (now available through the BioGPS portal http://biogps.gnf.org/) which quantifies mRNA expression pattern based on microarray technique. Specifically, the Allen Brain Atlas has been used to retrieve two different mRNA quantifications from 17 different brain regions: the expression density and the expression level (http://mouse.brain-map. org/pdf/InformaticsDataProcessing.pdf) [14]. The expression level (L) is the product of the average pixel intensity (I) per total area of positive cells (ag), normalized by the area of all cells (amax): L = I ⁎ (ag/amax). The expression density (D) is the number of positive cells (ng) normalized by the total number of cells (nmax) in the same region: D = ng/nmax. The expressions L and D were already quantified by Allen Brain Atlas database for the following brain regions: cerebellum (CB), cerebral cortex (CTX), hippocampal region (HIP), hippocampal formation (HPF = HIP + subiculum), hypothalamus (HY), lateral septal complex (LSX), midbrain (MB), medulla (MY), olfactory areas (OLF = olfactory bulb, anterior olfactory nuclei, and pyriform cortex), Pons (P), Pallidum (PAL), retrohippocampal region (RHP), striatum (STR), thalamus (TH), striatum-like amygdalar nuclei (AMY= central+ anterior + medial amygdalar nucleus). Future studies will address possible subdivisions of these brain regions (in particular the cerebral cortex) in order to obtain spatially defined results. The SymAtlas database has been used to retrieve the mRNA expression value from the following brain regions: CB, CTX, HIP, HY, substantia nigra (SN), OLF, STR, AMY, frontal cortex (FC), medial olfactory epithelium (MOB), preoptic nucleus (PO), and spinal cord (SC). The database used (Mouse GNF1M Gene Atlas) reported the mRNA expression from multiple probes normalized with the Robust Multi-array Analysis (gcmrna algorithm) [15,16]. Data were submitted to two-way analysis of variance group × brain region separately for the two databases. Posthoc comparisons were
Calcium channel, voltage-dependent, beta 4 subunit (Cacnb4) [22] Dopamine beta hydroxylase (Dbh) [26,27] D site albumin promoter binding protein (Dbp) [30] Dopamine receptor 2 (Drd2) [33] Dopamine Receptor 4(Drd4) [36] Engrailed 1 (En1) [40] Engrailed 2 (En2) [40] Gastrin (Gast) [46] Glutamate receptor, metabotropic 8 (Grm8) [49] Orexin (Hcrt) [52] Hdc [55,56] Htr1a [59] Potassium inwardly rectifying channel, subfamily J, member 11, Kir 6.2 (Kcnj11) [61] Yamaguchi sarcoma viral (v-yes-1) oncogene homolog (Lyn) [64] Methyl CpG binding protein 2 (MeCP2) [67] Npy [70] Soluble adenylyl cyclase (sacy) [73] Solute carrier family 6 (neurotransmitter transporter, noradrenalin), member 2, NET (Slc6a2) [76,77] Serotonin Transporter, SERT (Slc6a4) [40,80–82] Somatostatin receptor-2 (sstr2) [85] Tyrosine Hydroxylase (Th) [89–93] Tyroid Hormone Receptor alpha (Thra) [96] Ubiquitin protein ligase E3 component n-recognin 1 (Ubr1) [98] Vitamin D receptor (Vdr) [101]
ATP-binding cassette, subfamily A (ABC1), member 2 (Abca2) [21] Adenylate cyclase activating polypeptide 1, PACAP (Adcyap1) [24,25] Adaptor-related protein complex 3, delta 1 subunit (Ap3d1) [29] Arylsulfatase A (Arsa or Asa) [32] ATPase, Na+/K + transporting, alpha 3 polypeptide (Atp1a3) [35] Calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II alpha (Camk2a) [39] Cholinergic receptor, muscarinic 1, CNS (Chrm1) [42,43] Cyclic nucleotide phosphodiesterase 1 (Cnp1) [45] Dopamine receptor D1A (Drd1a) [47,48] Fibroblast growth factor 1 and 2 (Fgf1 and Fgf2) [51] Fibroblast growth factor receptor 1 (Fgfr1) [54] Fragile X mental retardation syndrome 1 homolog (Fmr1) [58] Fragile X mental retardation gene 2, autosomal homolog (Fxr2h) [60]
Glast (Slc1a3) [79] glutamate receptor, ionotropic, AMPA1 (alpha 1) (Gria1) [84] Glutamate receptor, ionotropic, NMDA1 (zeta 1) (Grin1) [87,88] Glycogen synthase kinase 3 beta (Gsk3b) [95] 5-hydroxytryptamine (serotonin) receptor 2C (Htr2c) [18] Inositol (myo)-1(or 4)-monophosphatase 1 (Impa1) [100] Kv3.1, potassium voltage-gated channel, Shaw-related subfamily, member 1, Potassium channel (Kcnc1) [102,103] Kv3.3, potassium voltage-gated channel, Shaw-related subfamily, member 3 (Kcnc3) [102,103] GIRK2, potassium inwardly rectifying channel, subfamily J (Kcnj6) [17] Potassium voltage-gated channel, subfamily Q, member 2 (Kcnq2) [104] ERK1, mitogen activated protein kinase 3 (Mapk3) [105] Melanin-concentrating hormone receptor 1 (Mchr1) [106] Microtubule-associated protein 6 or Stable Tubule-only peptide (STOP) (Mtap6) [107,108] NFkB-p50 subunit (Nfkb1) [109] Nur77, Nuclear receptor subfamily 4, group A, member 1 (Nr4a1) [110] Nurr1, nuclear receptor subfamily 4, group A, member 2 (Nr4a2) [111–113] Heregulin (Nrg1) [114] Protein-L-isoaspartate (D-aspartate) O-methyltransferase 1 (Pcmt1) [115] p85alpha (Pik3r1)[116] Prion protein (Prnp) [117] Ryr3 [118] Slc2a8 [119] DAT1, solute carrier family 6 (neurotransmitter transporter, dopamine), member 3 (Slc6a3) [120–122] Synaptosomal-associated protein 25 (Snap25) [123–126] Synapsin2 (Syn2) [127] Tropomodulin 2 (Tmod2) [128] Tenascin R, (Tnr) [129]
Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA-B) receptor, 1 (Gabbr1) [63] Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) B receptor 2 (Gabbr2) [66] Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA-A) receptor, subunit alpha 3 (Gabra3) [69] Growth associated protein 43 (Gap43) [72] Connexin 43, gap junction membrane channel protein alpha 1 (Gja1) [75]
Locomotor activity decreased more than 20% when the gene is deleted (group 2)
Locomotor activity increased more than 20% when the gene is deleted (group 1)
Table 1 Genes related to locomotor behaviour.
P2rx7 [83] Cyclophilin D (Ppid) [86] Relaxin3 (Rln3) [94] Solute carrier family 32 (GABA vesicular transporter), member 1 (Slc32a1) [97] Sv2a [99]
Connexin 36, gap junction membrane channel protein alpha 9 (Gja9) [65] Glucagon-like peptide 1 receptor (Glp1r) [68] G-substrate (Gsbs) [71] Nr cam (C130076O07Rik) [74] Neuropsin (Opn5) [78]
Acetylcholine esterase (AChE) [23] Adrenergic receptor, alpha 2a (Adra2a) [28] Calpstatin (Cast) [31] Cav1.3 (cacna1d) [34] Cell adhesion molecule with homology to L1CAM (Chl1) [37,38] Muscarinic receptor M2 (Chrm2) [41] Cholinergic receptor, nicotinic, beta 2 subunit (neuronal) (Chrnb2) [44] Claudin 14 (cldn 14) Aromatase (Cyp19a1) [50] Egr2 [53] Forkhead Box, Class O (Foxo3a) [57] Galanin (Gal) Galanin receptor 2 (Galr2) [62]
Locomotor activity changes smaller than 10% (not significant) when the gene is deleted (“control” group)
620 P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
done using LSD test for repeated comparisons. Rejection level was set at p b 0.05. 3. Results Using the inclusion/exclusion criteria described above 93 knockout mice have been selected: 46 knockout mice with increased locomotor behavior, 24 knockout mice with decreased locomotor behavior and 23 knockout mice with other types of neurological impairments but normal locomotor behavior (Table 1). Five additional knockout mice were excluded from the analysis because behavioral reports from the literature were not consistent (cnr1, cckbr and bdnf) or were identified as outliers on the basis of the brain expression level (Pde10A) or their involvement in behavioral abnormalities were probably mediated by systemic metabolic alterations (pah). In agreement with a previous report, genes related to hyperactive and hypoactive behavior could not be grouped into separate neurotransmitter classes. Similarly, genes of the control “normoactive” group belonged to neurotransmitters/neuropeptides (galanin) or structural proteins (Gja9, Nr CAM, and L1CAM), or different neurotransmitter systems (epinephrine, acetylcholine, and GABA) and thus could not be grouped in only one neurotransmitter or cellular system. Therefore, the knowledge of the function of a specific gene at cellular or neuronal level does not carry enough information to predict if its deletion might change locomotor behavior. In contrast, a better predictor of gene deletion effect on locomotor behavior might be the level of expression of the same gene in the brain (mRNA expression). To test this hypothesis the expression levels of specific genes were analyzed in different brain regions in order to have predictable indicators on locomotor behavior after gene deletion, using two different datasets. In the following description the term “group 1 genes” is referred to the group of genes that, after deletion, are accompanied by hyperactivity, “group 2 genes” is referred to the group of genes that, after deletion, are accompanied by hypoactivity and “control genes” will refer to the group of genes that, after deletion, are not accompanied by changes in locomotor activity. Since the genetic background was not the same for all KO animals selected, we tested if a bias in the background among the groups was present. The majority of the KO animals were raised in C57BL/6 or Sv 129 background or in a mixed C57BL/6 × Sv129 background. Few animals in our set were studied in other genetic backgrounds such as balb/c (n = 4), icr (n = 1), FVB (n = 2) or DBA (n = 2). Therefore, to test the homogeneity of the groups we focused on the three major backgrounds (C57BL/6Sv129 and mixed C57BL/6 × Sv129). As shown in Fig. 1, these backgrounds were similarly represented in the groups divided according the behavioral activity. In fact, a non-parametric χ2 test of the observed frequency distribution of the three backgrounds in the three behavioral groups was not statistically significant. The analysis of the brain expression pattern of these genes is shown in Fig. 1. Group 1 genes showed higher expression level and expression density in all brain regions, compared to group 2 genes (Figs. 1 and 2). The brain expression pattern of group 1 and group 2 genes were then compared to the group of control genes. This analysis showed that, when compared to the control genes, the genes of group 1 showed significantly higher expression (level and density) in all brain regions according to the Allen Brain Atlas (p b b 0.01 in all brain regions). However, using expression leves from the SymAtlas database, only in the following regions were significant (Fig. 1): (i) the cerebral cortex (CTX; p = 0.041, LSD test for repeated comparisons), (ii) the hippocampus (HIP, p = 0.035, LSD test for repeated comparisons), and (iii) the amygdala (AMY, p = 0.030, LSD test for repeated comparisons). The group 2 genes did not show significant changes from the control group in any brain region according to the Allen Brain Atlas dataset and the SymAtlas dataset.
621
A finer analysis was then conducted to verify the relationship between the expression level of each gene in the hippocampus and the change in locomotor activity resulting from their deletion. To this aim, rather than classifying knockout animals in three groups (increased, decreased or normal locomotor activity), the locomotor activity of each knockout animal compared to the appropriate wild type control littermate (expressed as percent of basal value) was retrieved from literature data. The locomotor activity level was then plotted against the expression level in the hippocampus of each gene according to the Allen Brain Atlas and the Sym Atlas dataset. Fig. 1 shows a significant correlation between the expression density of different genes in the hippocampus and the locomotor activity level resulting after deletion of the same genes (r = 0.542 according to the Allen Brain Atlas and r = 0.502 according to the SymAtlas database). Thus, the higher the expression of a gene in the hippocampus, the higher the observed locomotor behavior when the same gene is deleted (Figs. 1 and 2). 4. Discussion This article describes the brain expression pattern of genes that are accompanied by changes in locomotor activity when deleted and compares it to the expression pattern of genes expressed in the brain, but whose deletion do not change the locomotor behavior in a novel environment (Fig. 2). The main finding is that the genes accompanied, after deletion, by increased locomotor activity (group 1 genes), are also much more expressed in all brain regions compared to genes accompanied by decreased locomotor behavior (group 2 genes). In order to understand if this effect was due to a lower expression level of group 2 genes or to a higher expression level of group 1 genes, both groups have been compared to a “control” group consisting of genes expressed in the brain, whose deletion do not alter locomotor activity. The comparative analysis of two brain atlases showed that group 1 genes were highly expressed in few brain regions, as the cerebral cortex, hippocampus and amygdala, compared to the “control” genes. Several aspects should be considered in the interpretation of the results. One important issue is the influence of experimental conditions in behavioral findings. As highlighted by previous articles (see e.g. [5]), laboratory conditions greatly change not only the effect size of a specific genetic mutation, but even the possibility to see a behavioral change. Indeed, most of the noise in correlation analysis showed in Fig. 1 is probably due to the differences among laboratories (for example lighting conditions might be very important for novelty-induced locomotor activity). However, the general conclusions — that is the higher the brain expression of a gene (and specifically the expression in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus) the higher the probability that its deletion will be accompanied by hyperactive behavior — appear to be quite robust notwithstanding this experimental noise. Specifically, the main conclusion remained unchanged even after the inclusion of new knockout animals in the database or after the exclusion of some of them due to inconsistencies in new publications or even using two independent gene atlases. A second issue is related to the study of hypoactive animals. As showed by Fig. 1, the maximum effect size in hypoactive animals is about half of the maximum effect size in hyperactive animals. This was expected, because hypoactivity can rapidly reach a minimum, consistent in the complete immobility of the animal (floor effect). This might challenge the possibility to detect hypoactivity, particularly if the wt littermates show already low levels of locomotor activity. Therefore, it is possible to hypothesize that genetic backgrounds with a higher basal locomotor activity (such as C57BL/6) might be more useful to detect hypoactive behavior (and, vice versa, as suggested by anonymous referee, the background Sv129 or FVB, which show lower
622
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
Fig. 1. Brain distribution and level of expression of genes whose deletion is accompanied by changes in locomotor activity. A: distribution of the genetic background according to the change of the locomotor activity as percent of control animals (0 = no behavioral changes, + 100/− 100 = 100% increase/decrease of locomotor activity compared to wt littermates; each dot represent a different knockout line). B,C Expression density according to the Allen Brain Atlas (B) and expression level according to the SymAtlas (C) of genes whose deletion is accompanied by hyperactive (hyper) or hypoactive (hypo) behavior or does not change locomotor activity (normal) in different brain regions. Each point represents the average expression of the genes that, when deleted, are accompanied by hyperactivity/hypoactivity or do not change locomotor behavior (see Table 1 for a list of the genes); D: statistical significance (p-value using multiple LSD comparisons) of the increase in expression level according to the SymAtlas (horizontal axis) and Allen Brain Atlas (vertical axis) of hyperactive genes compared to control genes in various brain regions (each dot represent a brain region). D: Non-linear monotonic correlation between the locomotor activity (0 = normal behavior) of knockout animals and the expression density in the cerebral cortex of the genes knocked out (each dot represents a different gene). B,C data represent mean ± standard error. CB: cerebellum; CTX: cerebral cortex; HIP: hippocampus; HPF: hippocampal formation; HY: hypothalamus; LSX: lateral septel complex; MB: midbrain; MY: medulla; OLF: olfactory regions; P: Pons; PAL: Pallidum; RHP: retrohippocampal region; STR: striatum (dorsal/ventral); TH: thalamus; AMY: striatum-like amygdalar nuclei, SN: substantia nigra; MOB: medial olfactory epithelium; PO: preoptic; FC: frontal cortex; SC: spinal cord.
activity in a novel environment, might be useful to detect a hyperactive behavior due to gene deletion). We did not observe a particular bias of the genetic background used in the literature to observe hyperactive or hypoactive behavior (see Fig. 1). However, the smaller number of hypoactive animals might reflect a real difficulty to detect this type of behavior. Another issue is the consideration that most of the selected genes are also expressed in regions outside the brain, and therefore the behavioral effects of their deletion might be indirect, as metabolic changes. When more tissue-specific conditional knockout mice will be
available, this issue will be appropriately addressed. However, the results concerning the brain expression pattern of group 1 genes are consistent with data from lesion studies. In fact, cerebral cortex and hippocampal lesions have been shown to increase locomotor activity (reviewed in [13]). Therefore it is plausible that the deletion of genes highly expressed in these brain regions would also result in increased locomotor behavior. Accordingly, the deletion of a gene highly expressed in the cerebral cortex may account for an increase in locomotor activity, independently from its function at cellular/neuronal level. It should be noted that, if the
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
623
Fig. 2. Brain distribution of representative genes whose deletion is accompanied by an increase (left column), a decrease (right column) of locomotor activity in novel environments or does not change locomotor activity (central column). Data from the Allen Brain Atlas, reconstructed using the Brain Explorer software.
lack of a specific gene is accompanied by hyperactivity, the “normal” function of the same gene would be to decrease locomotor activity. For example hyperactivity emerges when two genes highly expressed in the brain such as girk2 [17] or serotonin receptor 2C genes [18] are deleted, and therefore they should be expected to decrease locomotor activity in normal mice, when normally expressed. Therefore, a working hypothesis is that genes highly expressed in the brain, and particularly in the cerebral cortex, have larger probability to be necessary for the development of a neural network which normally reduces locomotor activity [13]. It should be expected that focal lesions of different portions in this neuronal network either by pharmacological interventions (see [13] for a review) either by deletion/mutation of genes highly expressed in the same brain regions might alter the computational properties of this structure, a prerequisite for the development of novelty-induced hyperactivity. This specific type of hyperactivity depends on the recognition of previously explored situations, because in normal animals the exploration of familiar environments causes a reduction of the exploratory activity (habituation). Thus, if the brain regions, such as the cerebral cortex, involved in environmental recognition and mapping were altered by genetic modifications or pharmacological interventions, their function would be impaired, with consequent lower recognition ability and slower habituation to a novel environment. This is in agreement with previous reports that a large number of transgenic animals show altered hippocampal-dependent spatial memory [19]. Moreover, this neural network is expected to be mature at very early phases of life, since the capability of rat pups to explore is similar to adult rats [20]. An unresolved issue is whether gene expression alterations could be related to behavioral mechanisms like altered fear, reactivity to
reinforces. Indeed, further studies are currently in progress to retrieve knockout animals with these behavioral abnormalities, in order to study the statistical association with locomotor hyper/hypoactivity in the entire set of knockout mice. These results have implications for the understanding of gene– behavior relationship and for research in human psychiatric diseases. In fact, it appears that the action mechanism of a specific gene is less important than its expression level in specific brain regions in the prediction of its relevance for behavioral processes. Secondly, the increased locomotor activity accompanying mental diseases such as schizophrenia and ADHD, might be due to the brain expression level of the genes involved in the main symptoms of the disease (e.g. hallucinations and inattention, respectively). Specifically, it is plausible that if the genes implicated in the lack of attention (like in ADHD) or in hallucinations (like in schizophrenia) are highly expressed in the cortex or hippocampus, this might give rise to hyperactivity as showed in the present paper. Therefore, the occurring of hyperactivity in several psychiatric diseases might be explained as a collateral symptom due to alteration of genes highly expressed in the cerebral cortex or in the hippocampus.
Acknowledgments I am grateful to my wife Giovanna, for the encouragement and for the revision of the paper (DV). We are grateful to Dr. A. Bartolomucci and Dr. R. Gainetdinov who gave very useful suggestions. We also acknowledge the anonymous referees who greatly helped to improve the manuscript with their comments. We thank The Allen Institute for Brain Science for the permission to use their data in this publication.
624
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
Finally, we are grateful to Dr. D. Clement for useful discussions of the data, presented at the Academy of Sciences in Prague.
[31]
References
[32]
[1] Feder A, Nestler EJ, Charney DS. Psychobiology and molecular genetics of resilience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10:446–57. [2] Tretter F, Albus M. Systems biology and psychiatry — modeling molecular and cellular networks of mental disorders. Pharmacopsychiatry 2008;41(Suppl 1): S2–S18. [3] Lombroso C. L' uomo di Genio in Rapporto alla psichiatria, alla storia ed all'estetica. Torino; 1888. [4] Gould SJ. The ape in some of us: criminal anthropology. The Mismeasure of Man. New York: WW. Norton & Co; 1981. [5] Crabbe JC, Wahlsten D, Dudek BC. Genetics of mouse behavior: interactions with laboratory environment. Science 1999;284:1670–2. [6] Crusio WE, Goldowitz D, Holmes A, Wolfer D. Standards for the publication of mouse mutant studies. Genes Brain Behav 2008;8:1–4. [7] Anagnostopoulos AV, Mobraaten LE, Sharp JJ, Davisson MT. Transgenic and knockout databases: behavioral profiles of mouse mutants. Physiol Behav 2001;73:675–89. [8] Hancock JM, Adams NC, Aidinis V, Blake A, Bogue M, Brown SD, et al. Mouse Phenotype Database Integration Consortium: integration [corrected] of mouse phenome data resources. Mamm Genome 2007;18:157–63. [9] Richter SH, Garner JP, Wurbel H. Environmental standardization: cure or cause of poor reproducibility in animal experiments? Nat Methods 2009;6:257–61. [10] Curtis KS, Krause EG. Sex differences in physiology and behavior: focus on central actions of ovarian hormones. Physiol Behav 2009;97:141–2. [11] Thomas MB, Hu M, Lee TM, Bhatnagar S, Becker JB. Sex-specific susceptibility to cocaine in rats with a history of prenatal stress. Physiol Behav 2009;97:270–7. [12] Arndt SS, Laarakker MC, van Lith HA, van der Staay FJ, Gieling E, Salomons AR, et al. Individual housing of mice — impact on behaviour and stress responses. Physiol Behav 2009;97:385–93. [13] Viggiano D. The hyperactive syndrome: metaanalysis of genetic alterations, pharmacological treatments and brain lesions which increase locomotor activity. Behav Brain Res 2008;194:1–14. [14] Ng L, Bernard A, Lau C, Overly CC, Dong HW, Kuan C, et al. An anatomic gene expression atlas of the adult mouse brain. Nat Neurosci 2009;12:356–62. [15] Su AI, Cooke MP, Ching KA, Hakak Y, Walker JR, Wiltshire T, et al. Large-scale analysis of the human and mouse transcriptomes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:4465–70. [16] Su AI, Wiltshire T, Batalov S, Lapp H, Ching KA, Block D, et al. A gene atlas of the mouse and human protein-encoding transcriptomes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004;101:6062–7. [17] Blednov YA, Stoffel M, Cooper R, Wallace D, Mane N, Harris RA. Hyperactivity and dopamine D1 receptor activation in mice lacking girk2 channels. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;159:370–8. [18] Nonogaki K, Abdallah L, Goulding EH, Bonasera SJ, Tecott LH. Hyperactivity and reduced energy cost of physical activity in serotonin 5-HT(2C) receptor mutant mice. Diabetes 2003;52:315–20. [19] Wolfer DP, Stagljar-Bozicevic M, Errington ML, Lipp HP. Spatial memory and learning in transgenic mice: fact or artifact? News Physiol Sci 1998;13:118–23. [20] Smith KS, Morrell JI. Comparison of infant and adult rats in exploratory activity, diurnal patterns, and responses to novel and anxiety-provoking environments. Behav Neurosci 2007;121:449–61. [21] Mack JT, Beljanski V, Soulika AM, Townsend DM, Brown CB, Davis W, et al. “Skittish” abca2 knockout mice display tremor, hyperactivity, and abnormal myelin ultrastructure in the central nervous system. Mol Cell Biol 2007;27: 44–53. [22] Khan Z, Jinnah HA. Paroxysmal dyskinesias in the lethargic mouse mutant. J Neurosci 2002;22:8193–200. [23] Espallergues J, Galvan L, Sabatier F, Rana-Poussine V, Maurice T, Chatonnet A. Behavioral phenotyping of heterozygous acetylcholinesterase knockout (AChE+/−) mice showed no memory enhancement but hyposensitivity to amnesic drugs. Behav Brain Res 2010;206:263–73. [24] Tanaka K, Shintani N, Hashimoto H, Kawagishi N, Ago Y, Matsuda T, et al. Psychostimulant-induced attenuation of hyperactivity and prepulse inhibition deficits in Adcyap1-deficient mice. J Neurosci 2006;26:5091–7. [25] Fujii H, Ishihama T, Ago Y, Shintani N, Kakuda M, Hashimoto H, et al. Methamphetamine-induced hyperactivity and behavioral sensitization in PACAP deficient mice. Peptides 2007;28:1674–9. [26] Weinshenker D, Miller NS, Blizinsky K, Laughlin ML, Palmiter RD. Mice with chronic norepinephrine deficiency resemble amphetamine-sensitized animals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002;99:13873–7. [27] Schank JR, Ventura R, Puglisi-Allegra S, Alcaro A, Cole CD, Liles LC, et al. Dopamine beta-hydroxylase knockout mice have alterations in dopamine signaling and are hypersensitive to cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31:2221–30. [28] Juhila J, Honkanen A, Sallinen J, Haapalinna A, Korpi ER, Scheinin M. a2Aadrenoceptors regulate D-amphetamine-induced hyperactivity and behavioural sensitization in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 2005;517:74–83. [29] Miller CL, Burmeister M, Stevens KE. Hippocampal auditory gating in the hyperactive mocha mouse. Neurosci Lett 1999;276:57–60. [30] Le Niculescu H, McFarland MJ, Ogden CA, Balaraman Y, Patel S, Tan J, et al. Phenomic, convergent functional genomic, and biomarker studies in a stress-
[33]
[34]
[35]
[36]
[37]
[38] [39]
[40]
[41]
[42]
[43]
[44]
[45]
[46]
[47]
[48]
[49]
[50]
[51]
[52] [53]
[54]
[55]
[56]
reactive genetic animal model of bipolar disorder and co-morbid alcoholism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B:134–66. Nakajima R, Takao K, Huang SM, Takano J, Iwata N, Miyakawa T, et al. Comprehensive behavioral phenotyping of calpastatin-knockout mice. Mol Brain 2008;1:7. D'Hooge R, Van Dam D, Franck F, Gieselmann V, De Deyn PP. Hyperactivity, neuromotor defects, and impaired learning and memory in a mouse model for metachromatic leukodystrophy. Brain Res 2001;907:35–43. Yamaguchi H, Aiba A, Nakamura K, Nakao K, Sakagami H, Goto K, et al. Dopamine D2 receptor plays a critical role in cell proliferation and proopiomelanocortin expression in the pituitary. Genes Cells 1996;1:253–68. Busquet P, Khoi NN, Schmid E, Tanimoto N, Seeliger MW, Ben Yosef T, et al. CaV1.3 L-type Ca2+ channels modulate depression-like behaviour in mice independent of deaf phenotype. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009:1–15. Moseley AE, Williams MT, Schaefer TL, Bohanan CS, Neumann JC, Behbehani MM, et al. Deficiency in Na, K-ATPase alpha isoform genes alters spatial learning, motor activity, and anxiety in mice. J Neurosci 2007;27:616–26. Rubinstein M, Phillips TJ, Bunzow JR, Falzone TL, Dziewczapolski G, Zhang G, et al. Mice lacking dopamine D4 receptors are supersensitive to ethanol, cocaine, and methamphetamine. Cell 1997;90:991–1001. Morellini F, Lepsveridze E, Kahler B, Dityatev A, Schachner M. Reduced reactivity to novelty, impaired social behavior, and enhanced basal synaptic excitatory activity in perforant path projections to the dentate gyrus in young adult mice deficient in the neural cell adhesion molecule CHL1. Mol Cell Neurosci 2007;34: 121–36. Pratte M, Jamon M. Impairment of novelty detection in mice targeted for the Chl1 gene. Physiol Behav 2009;97:394–400. Chen C, Rainnie DG, Greene RW, Tonegawa S. Abnormal fear response and aggressive behavior in mutant mice deficient for alpha-calcium–calmodulin kinase II. Science 1994;266:291–4. Sgado P, Alberi L, Gherbassi D, Galasso SL, Ramakers GM, Alavian KN, et al. Slow progressive degeneration of nigral dopaminergic neurons in postnatal Engrailed mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006;103:15242–7. Bainbridge NK, Koselke LR, Jeon J, Bailey KR, Wess J, Crawley JN, et al. Learning and memory impairments in a congenic C57BL/6 strain of mice that lacks the M2 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtype. Behav Brain Res 2008;190:50–8. Miyakawa T, Yamada M, Duttaroy A, Wess J. Hyperactivity and intact hippocampusdependent learning in mice lacking the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor. J Neurosci 2001;21:5239–50. Gerber DJ, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Huang SY, Caron MG, Tonegawa S. Hyperactivity, elevated dopaminergic transmission, and response to amphetamine in M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:15312–7. Marubio LM, Gardier AM, Durier S, David D, Klink R, Arroyo-Jimenez MM, et al. Effects of nicotine in the dopaminergic system of mice lacking the alpha4 subunit of neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Eur J NeuroSci 2003;17: 1329–37. Kaga Y, Shoemaker WJ, Furusho M, Bryant M, Rosenbluth J, Pfeiffer SE, et al. Mice with conditional inactivation of fibroblast growth factor receptor-2 signaling in oligodendrocytes have normal myelin but display dramatic hyperactivity when combined with Cnp1 inactivation. J Neurosci 2006;26:12339–50. Singh P, Cobb S, Rengifo-Cam W, Deng X, Willis W, Li Q. Locomotor activity and behavior of mutant mice deleted for gastrin gene expression. J Physiol Pharmacol 2004;55:269–78. Xu M, Moratalla R, Gold LH, Hiroi N, Koob GF, Graybiel AM, et al. Dopamine D1 receptor mutant mice are deficient in striatal expression of dynorphin and in dopamine-mediated behavioral responses. Cell 1994;79:729–42. Karasinska JM, George SR, Cheng R, O'Dowd BF. Deletion of dopamine D1 and D3 receptors differentially affects spontaneous behaviour and cocaine-induced locomotor activity, reward and CREB phosphorylation. Eur J NeuroSci 2005;22: 1741–50. Duvoisin RM, Zhang C, Pfankuch TF, O'Connor H, Gayet-Primo J, Quraishi S, et al. Increased measures of anxiety and weight gain in mice lacking the group III metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR8. Eur J NeuroSci 2005;22:425–36. Chavez C, Gogos A, Jones ME, Van den BM. Psychotropic drug-induced locomotor hyperactivity and prepulse inhibition regulation in male and female aromatase knockout (ArKO) mice: role of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors and dopamine transporters. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;206:267–79. Fadda P, Bedogni F, Fresu A, Collu M, Racagni G, Riva MA. Reduction of corticostriatal glutamatergic fibers in basic fibroblast growth factor deficient mice is associated with hyperactivity and enhanced dopaminergic transmission. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62(3):235–42. Mori T, Ito S, Kuwaki T, Yanagisawa M, Sawaguchi T. Monoaminergic neuronal changes in orexin deficient mice. Neuropharmacology 2009. Poirier R, Cheval H, Mailhes C, Charnay P, Davis S, Laroche S. Paradoxical role of an egr transcription factor family member, egr2/krox20, in learning and memory. Front Behav Neurosci 2007;1:6. Shin DM, Korada S, Raballo R, Shashikant CS, Simeone A, Taylor JR, et al. Loss of glutamatergic pyramidal neurons in frontal and temporal cortex resulting from attenuation of FGFR1 signaling is associated with spontaneous hyperactivity in mice. J Neurosci 2004;24:2247–58. Acevedo SF, Ohtsu H, Benice TS, Rizk-Jackson A, Raber J. Age-dependent measures of anxiety and cognition in male histidine decarboxylase knockout (Hdc−/−) mice. Brain Res 2006;1071:113–23. Dere E, Souza-Silva MA, Spieler RE, Lin JS, Ohtsu H, Haas HL, et al. Changes in motoric, exploratory and emotional behaviours and neuronal acetylcholine
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
[57]
[58]
[59]
[60]
[61]
[62]
[63]
[64]
[65]
[66]
[67]
[68]
[69]
[70] [71]
[72]
[73]
[74]
[75]
[76]
[77]
[78]
[79]
[80] [81]
[82]
[83]
content and 5-HT turnover in histidine decarboxylase-KO mice. Eur J NeuroSci 2004;20:1051–8. Polter A, Yang S, Zmijewska AA, van Groen T, Paik JH, Depinho RA, et al. Forkhead box, class O transcription factors in brain: regulation and behavioral manifestation. Biol Psychiatry 2009;65:150–9. Ventura R, Pascucci T, Catania MV, Musumeci SA, Puglisi-Allegra S. Object recognition impairment in Fmr1 knockout mice is reversed by amphetamine: involvement of dopamine in the medial prefrontal cortex. Behav Pharmacol 2004;15:433–42. Zanettini C, Carola V, Lo IL, Moles A, Gross C, D'Amato FR. Postnatal handling reverses social anxiety in serotonin receptor 1A knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav 2009 PMID: 19740091. Bontekoe CJ, McIlwain KL, Nieuwenhuizen IM, Yuva-Paylor LA, Nellis A, Willemsen R, et al. Knockout mouse model for Fxr2: a model for mental retardation. Hum Mol Genet 2002;11:487–98. Deacon RM, Brook RC, Meyer D, Haeckel O, Ashcroft FM, Miki T, et al. Behavioral phenotyping of mice lacking the K ATP channel subunit Kir6.2. Physiol Behav 2006;87:723–33. Lu X, Ross B, Sanchez-Alavez M, Zorrilla EP, Bartfai T. Phenotypic analysis of GalR2 knockout mice in anxiety- and depression-related behavioral tests. Neuropeptides 2008;42:387–97. Vacher CM, Gassmann M, Desrayaud S, Challet E, Bradaia A, Hoyer D, et al. Hyperdopaminergia and altered locomotor activity in GABAB1-deficient mice. J Neurochem 2006;97:979–91. Umemori H, Ogura H, Tozawa N, Mikoshiba K, Nishizumi H, Yamamoto T. Impairment of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor-controlled motor activity in LYNdeficient mice. Neuroscience 2003;118:709–13. Frisch C, De Souza Silva MA, Sohl G, Guldenagel M, Willecke K, Huston JP, et al. Stimulus compleity dependent memory impairment and changes in motor performance after deletion of the neuronal gap junction protein connexin36 in mice. Behav Brain Res 2005;157:177–85. Gassmann M, Shaban H, Vigot R, Sansig G, Haller C, Barbieri S, et al. Redistribution of GABAB(1) protein and atypical GABAB responses in GABAB(2)-deficient mice. J Neurosci 2004;24:6086–97. Shahbazian M, Young J, Yuva-Paylor L, Spencer C, Antalffy B, Noebels J, et al. Mice with truncated MeCP2 recapitulate many Rett syndrome features and display hyperacetylation of histone H3. Neuron 2002;35:243–54. Abbas T, Faivre E, Holscher C. Impairment of synaptic plasticity and memory formation in GLP-1 receptor KO mice: Interaction between type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer's disease. Behav Brain Res 2009;205:265–71. Yee BK, Keist R, von Boehmer L, Studer R, Benke D, Hagenbuch N, et al. A schizophrenia-related sensorimotor deficit links alpha 3-containing GABAA receptors to a dopamine hyperfunction. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005;102: 17154–9. Karl T, Duffy L, Herzog H. Behavioural profile of a new mouse model for NPY deficiency. Eur J NeuroSci 2008;28:173–80. Endo S, Shutoh F, Dinh TL, Okamoto T, Ikeda T, Suzuki M, et al. Dual involvement of G-substrate in motor learning revealed by gene deletion. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:3525–30. Metz GA, Schwab ME. Behavioral characterization in a comprehensive mouse test battery reveals motor and sensory impairments in growth-associated protein-43 null mutant mice. Neuroscience 2004;129:563–74. Iwamoto T, Okumura S, Iwatsubo K, Kawabe J, Ohtsu K, Sakai I, et al. Motor dysfunction in type 5 adenylyl cyclase-null mice. J Biol Chem 2003;278: 16936–40. Moy SS, Nonneman RJ, Young NB, Demyanenko GP, Maness PF. Impaired sociability and cognitive function in Nrcam-null mice. Behav Brain Res 2009;205: 123–31. Theis M, Jauch R, Zhuo L, Speidel D, Wallraff A, Doring B, et al. Accelerated hippocampal spreading depression and enhanced locomotory activity in mice with astrocyte-directed inactivation of connexin43. J Neurosci 2003;23:766–76. Mitchell HA, Ahern TH, Liles LC, Javors MA, Weinshenker D. The effects of norepinephrine transporter inactivation on locomotor activity in mice. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:1046–52. Dziedzicka-Wasylewska M, Faron-Gorecka A, Kusmider M, Drozdowska E, Rogoz Z, Siwanowicz J, et al. Effect of antidepressant drugs in mice lacking the norepinephrine transporter. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31:2424–32. Horii Y, Yamasaki N, Miyakawa T, Shiosaka S. Increased anxiety-like behavior in neuropsin (kallikrein-related peptidase 8) gene-deficient mice. Behav Neurosci 2008;122:498–504. Karlsson RM, Tanaka K, Heilig M, Holmes A. Loss of glial glutamate and aspartate transporter (excitatory amino acid transporter 1) causes locomotor hyperactivity and exaggerated responses to psychotomimetics: rescue by haloperidol and metabotropic glutamate 2/3 agonist. Biol Psychiatry 2008;64:810–4. Holmes A, Murphy DL, Crawley JN. Reduced aggression in mice lacking the serotonin transporter. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;161:160–7. Kalueff AV, Jensen CL, Murphy DL. Locomotory patterns, spatiotemporal organization of exploration and spatial memory in serotonin transporter knockout mice. Brain Res 2007;1169:87–97. Kalueff AV, Fox MA, Gallagher PS, Murphy DL. Hypolocomotion, anxiety and serotonin syndrome-like behavior contribute to the complex phenotype of serotonin transporter knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav 2007;6:389–400. Basso AM, Bratcher NA, Harris RR, Jarvis MF, Decker MW, Rueter LE. Behavioral profile of P2X7 receptor knockout mice in animal models of depression and anxiety: relevance for neuropsychiatric disorders. Behav Brain Res 2009;198: 83–90.
625
[84] Cowen MS, Schroff KC, Gass P, Sprengel R, Spanagel R. Neurobehavioral effects of alcohol in AMPA receptor subunit (GluR1) deficient mice. Neuropharmacology 2003;45:325–33. [85] Viollet C, Vaillend C, Videau C, Bluet-Pajot MT, Ungerer A, L'Heritier A, et al. Involvement of sst2 somatostatin receptor in locomotor, exploratory activity and emotional reactivity in mice. Eur J NeuroSci 2000;12:3761–70. [86] Luvisetto S, Basso E, Petronilli V, Bernardi P, Forte M. Enhancement of anxiety, facilitation of avoidance behavior, and occurrence of adult-onset obesity in mice lacking mitochondrial cyclophilin D. Neuroscience 2008;155:585–96. [87] Dorval KM, Wigg KG, Crosbie J, Tannock R, Kennedy JL, Ickowicz A, et al. Association of the glutamate receptor subunit gene GRIN2B with attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder. Genes Brain Behav 2007;6:444–52. [88] Duncan GE, Moy SS, Lieberman JA, Koller BH. Typical and atypical antipsychotic drug effects on locomotor hyperactivity and deficits in sensorimotor gating in a genetic model of NMDA receptor hypofunction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2006;85:481–91. [89] Viggiano D, Ruocco LA, Arcieri S, Sadile AG. Involvement of norepinephrine in the control of activity and attentive processes in animal models of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neural Plast 2004;11:133–49. [90] Hommel JD, Sears RM, Georgescu D, Simmons DL, DiLeone RJ. Local gene knockdown in the brain using viral-mediated RNA interference. Nat Med 2003;9: 1539–44. [91] Salahpour A, Medvedev IO, Beaulieu JM, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG. Local knockdown of genes in the brain using small interfering RNA: a phenotypic comparison with knockout animals. Biol Psychiatry 2007;61:65–9. [92] Kobayashi K, Morita S, Sawada H, Mizuguchi T, Yamada K, Nagatsu I, et al. Targeted disruption of the tyrosine hydroxylase locus results in severe catecholamine depletion and perinatal lethality in mice. J Biol Chem 1995;270: 27235–43. [93] Hnasko TS, Perez FA, Scouras AD, Stoll EA, Gale SD, Luquet S, et al. Cre recombinase-mediated restoration of nigrostriatal dopamine in dopaminedeficient mice reverses hypophagia and bradykinesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006;103:8858–63. [94] Smith CM, Lawrence AJ, Sutton SW, Gundlach AL. Behavioral phenotyping of mixed background (129 S5:B6) relaxin-3 knockout mice. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1160:236–41. [95] Prickaerts J, Moechars D, Cryns K, Lenaerts I, van Craenendonck H, Goris I, et al. Transgenic mice overexpressing glycogen synthase kinase 3beta: a putative model of hyperactivity and mania. J Neurosci 2006;26:9022–9. [96] Wilcoxon JS, Nadolski GJ, Samarut J, Chassande O, Redei EE. Behavioral inhibition and impaired spatial learning and memory in hypothyroid mice lacking thyroid hormone receptor alpha. Behav Brain Res 2007;177:109–16. [97] Chiu CS, Brickley S, Jensen K, Southwell A, Mckinney S, Cull-Candy S, et al. GABA transporter deficiency causes tremor, ataxia, nervousness, and increased GABAinduced tonic conductance in cerebellum. J Neurosci 2005;25:3234–45. [98] Balogh SA, McDowell CS, Denenberg VH. Behavioral characterization of mice lacking the ubiquitin ligase UBR1 of the N-end rule pathway. Genes Brain Behav 2002;1:223–9. [99] Lamberty Y, Detrait E, Leclercq K, Michel A, De Ryck M. Behavioural phenotyping reveals anxiety-like features of SV2A deficient mice. Behav Brain Res 2009;198: 329–33. [100] Cryns K, Shamir A, Van Acker N, Levi I, Daneels G, Goris I, et al. IMPA1 is essential for embryonic development and lithium-like pilocarpine sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33:674–84. [101] Burne TH, McGrath JJ, Eyles DW, Mackay-Sim A. Behavioural characterization of vitamin D receptor knockout mice. Behav Brain Res 2005;157:299–308. [102] Joho RH, Street C, Matsushita S, Knopfel T. Behavioral motor dysfunction in Kv3-type potassium channel-deficient mice. Genes Brain Behav 2006;5:472–82. [103] Espinosa F, Marks G, Heintz N, Joho RH. Increased motor drive and sleep loss in mice lacking Kv3-type potassium channels. Genes Brain Behav 2004;3:90–100. [104] Peters HC, Hu H, Pongs O, Storm JF, Isbrandt D. Conditional transgenic suppression of M channels in mouse brain reveals functions in neuronal excitability, resonance and behavior. Nat Neurosci 2005;8:51–60. [105] Engel SR, Creson TK, Hao Y, Shen Y, Maeng S, Nekrasova T, et al. The extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributes to the control of behavioral excitement. Mol Psychiatry 2009;14:448–61. [106] Smith DG, Tzavara ET, Shaw J, Luecke S, Wade M, Davis R, et al. Mesolimbic dopamine super-sensitivity in melanin-concentrating hormone-1 receptordeficient mice. J Neurosci 2005;25:914–22. [107] Fradley RL, O'Meara GF, Newman RJ, Andrieux A, Job D, Reynolds DS. STOP knockout and NMDA NR1 hypomorphic mice exhibit deficits in sensorimotor gating. Behav Brain Res 2005;163:257–64. [108] Begou M, Brun P, Bertrand JB, Job D, Schweitzer A, D'Amato T, et al. Post-pubertal emergence of alterations in locomotor activity in stop null mice. Synapse 2007;61:689–97. [109] Kassed CA, Herkenham M. NF-kappaB p50-deficient mice show reduced anxietylike behaviors in tests of exploratory drive and anxiety. Behav Brain Res 2004;154:577–84. [110] Gilbert F, Morissette M, St Hilaire M, Paquet B, Rouillard C, Di Paolo T, et al. Nur77 gene knockout alters dopamine neuron biochemical activity and dopamine turnover. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:538–47. [111] Jiang C, Wan X, He Y, Pan T, Jankovic J, Le W. Age-dependent dopaminergic dysfunction in Nurr1 knockout mice. Exp Neurol 2005;191:154–62. [112] Eells JB, Lipska BK, Yeung SK, Misler JA, Nikodem VM. Nurr1-null heterozygous mice have reduced mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine levels and increased stress-induced locomotor activity. Behav Brain Res 2002;136:267–75.
626
P. Mignogna, D. Viggiano / Physiology & Behavior 99 (2010) 618–626
[113] Backman C, You ZB, Perlmann T, Hoffer BJ. Elevated locomotor activity without altered striatal dopamine contents in Nurr1 heterozygous mice after acute exposure to methamphetamine. Behav Brain Res 2003;143:95–100. [114] Gerlai R, Pisacane P, Erickson S. Heregulin, but not ErbB2 or ErbB3, heterozygous mutant mice exhibit hyperactivity in multiple behavioral tasks. Behav Brain Res 2000;109:219–27. [115] Vitali R, Clarke S. Improved rotorod performance and hyperactivity in mice deficient in a protein repair methyltransferase. Behav Brain Res 2004;153: 129–41. [116] Tohda C, Nakanishi R, Kadowaki M. Hyperactivity, memory deficit and anxietyrelated behaviors in mice lacking the p85alpha subunit of phosphoinositide-3 kinase. Brain Dev 2009;31:69–74. [117] Valenti P, Cozzio A, Nishida N, Wolfer DP, Sakaguchi S, Lipp HP. Similar target, different effects: late-onset ataxia and spatial learning in prion protein-deficient mouse lines. Neurogenetics 2001;3:173–84. [118] Katsuta K, Umemura K, Ueyama N, Matsuoka N. Pharmacological evidence for a correlation between hippocampal CA1 cell damage and hyperlocomotion following global cerebral ischemia in gerbils. Eur J Pharmacol 2003;467:103–9. [119] Schmidt S, Gawlik V, Holter SM, Augustin R, Scheepers A, Behrens M, et al. Deletion of glucose transporter GLUT8 in mice increases locomotor activity. Behav Genet 2008;38:396–406. [120] Gainetdinov R, Wetsel W, Jones S, Levin E, Jaber M, Caron M. Role of serotonine in the paradoxical calming effect of psychostimulants on hyperactivity. Science 1999;283:397–401.
[121] Ralph-Williams RJ, Paulus MP, Zhuang X, Hen R, Geyer MA. Valproate attenuates hyperactive and perseverative behaviors in mutant mice with a dysregulated dopamine system. Biol Psychiatry 2003;53:352–9. [122] Zhuang X, Oosting RS, Jones SR, Gainetdinov RR, Miller GW, Caron MG, et al. Hyperactivity and impaired response habituation in hyperdopaminergic mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:1982–7. [123] Wilson MC. Coloboma mouse mutant as an animal model of hyperkinesis and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2000;24:51–8. [124] Jones MD, Hess EJ. Norepinephrine regulates locomotor hyperactivity in the mouse mutant coloboma. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;75:209–16. [125] Jones MD, Hess EJ. Norepinephrine regulates locomotor hyperactivity in the mouse mutant coloboma. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2003;75:209–16. [126] Jones MD, Williams ME, Hess EJ. Abnormal presynaptic catecholamine regulation in a hyperactive SNAP-25-deficient mouse mutant. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2001;68:669–76. [127] Dyck BA, Skoblenick KJ, Castellano JM, Ki K, Thomas N, Mishra RK. Behavioral abnormalities in synapsin II knockout mice implicate a causal factor in schizophrenia. Synapse 2009;63:662–72. [128] Cox PR, Fowler V, Xu B, Sweatt JD, Paylor R, Zoghbi HY. Mice lacking Tropomodulin-2 show enhanced long-term potentiation, hyperactivity, and deficits in learning and memory. Mol Cell Neurosci 2003;23:1–12. [129] Fukamauchi F, Wang YJ, Mataga N, Kusakabe M. Paradoxical behavioral response to apomorphine in tenascin-gene knockout mouse. Eur J Pharmacol 1997;338: 7–10.