Chimpanzee Subsistence Technology: Materials and Skills
Geza Teleki Department of Anthropology, Pennsylvania State University, 409 Social Sciences Building, University Park, Pa. 16802, U.S.A.
Beyond reviewing basic data on Primate technological behavior, the aim of this report is to document the nature of chimpanzee technical skills by examining some of the mental as well as physical expertise which chimpanzees bring to bear on subsistence activities. This task is approached along several avenues : first, an outline is drawn of current knowledge about chimpanzee subsistence technology throughout Africa; second, a sketch is made of technological variability in several chimpanzee populations; third, the results of a personal investigation into the skills needed by chimpanzees to probe for insects are provided; and fourth, a comparison is made of the baboon, chimpanzee and human techniques used to exploit termites as a food resource. Instead of focusing on the unique features of human subsistence technology, the report attempts to show that many technical skills are and probably were firmly rooted in Primate prehistory, well before the advent of the earliest hominids. An integrated model of technological achievements among extant Primates, based on a sample ofAfrican cercopithecid, pongid and hominid populations representing stages in a phylogentic sequence, is offered as a foundation for reconstructing the gradual evolution of primate subsistence technology. This approach is intended to provoke contemplation and discussion among those investigators of human behavior and society who favor the thesis that the criterion of technology, together with the cultural transmission of technical skills, separates the human from the nonhuman Primates.
Received 15 July 1974 and accepted 8 August 1974
1. Introduction Research
on the technological
a century.
capabilities
of captive
for use in specific
tasks range
from
to the spontaneous
the designed
Kijhler
(1925)
1973).
It was merely a decade ago, however,
yield information 1964;
on their
Kortlandt,
The
chimpanzees
Studies on the ability of pongids to manipulate, behavior
technological
experiments
observed
in compounds
that research
performances
describing
such technological
using” and “tool-making”
performances
fauna
Kortlandt
1963,
1965;
serve as an introduction
achievements
observed
& Kooij,
conditions
by
(1972,
began to (Goodall,
skills exhibited
and on the materials
and specifically
on those attributes
activities-namely,
any behavior
and/or
substances
preparing
among nonhuman
1963;
Primates
van Lawick-Goodall,
and other
1970) should
Only the salient features of nonhuman Primate My attention will center on the will be considered here.
technical
patterns of behavior nests with branches, attention. Three main points
is now expanding at such Thus, reviews of the “tool-
to the subject.
behavior”
habitats,
oriented, for
utilized by chimpanzees
underlie
via technological
ingestion.
this limited
living in natural
which occur in association Due
involving objects (e.g. displacement grooming
years ofwork with chimpanzees
to
extension,
this
with subsistence toward obtaining
orientation,
many
other
drumming on buttress roots, building with leaves) will receive only cursory
focus.
in Gombe National
One,
having
Park, Tanzania,
Joumal of Human Evolution (1974) 3,575-594 10
in cages
by Menzel
on wild chimpanzees
in natural
a rate that all the data cannot be reviewed in this briefreport.
“technological
and modify objects
conducted
1965).
literature
(Hall,
spans more than half
assemble
completed
nearly
two
these field experiences
576
G.
TELEKI
have inevitably funneled my interests and insights in the direction of naturalistic behavior. Two, laboratory research on the skills elicited from chimpanzees and other Primates can provide vital information about the behavioral and psychological potentialities associated with manipulation and modification of objects, but only field research can establish the extent to which these potential capabilities are actually realized in natural populations. And three, knowledge about the realized subsistence technology of extant chimpanzee populations may shed light, albeit indirectly, on the evolution of technical skills among Primates in general and among the early hominids in particular. 2. Contexts
of Technological
Behavior
In order to provide some comparative perspective to the study of chimpanzee subsistence technology, the general dimensions and contexts of both baboon and chimpanzee technical skills are outlined below. A sketch of technological behavior in these African genera is appropriate for the following practical and theoretical reasons: because (a) many populations within these genera have been thoroughly studied in field conditions (Baldwin & Teleki, 1972, 1973); (b) the basic subsistence patterns of baboons and chimpanzees are similar, both being omnivores that collect flora and prey on fauna, and their collectorpredator habits can be ranked along a graded spectrum of diversity and complexity which integrate with human gatherer-hunter habits in the same African regions (Teleki, in press) ; (c) numerous populations of these genera live in areas that lie within or near to the African rift system, in habitats that range from arid savanna to rain forest, and thus inhabit geographical and ecological zones which were associated with hominid emergence (e.g. Pfeiffer, 1969); and because (d) these more than any other Primate genera have been cited as models for reconstructing the behavior, social life and subsistence habits of early hominids (e.g. Washburn & DeVore, 1961; Mann, 1972). In comparison to humans and even to chimpanzees, wild baboons exhibit a distinctly limited repertoire of technological behavior. Although Primates in general tend to apply their technical skills mainly in contexts other than subsistence (Hall, 1963 ; Kortlandt & Kooij, 1963), the meager records of technical performance among baboons are in fact restricted to this one context. Marais (1969) observed chacma baboons break open hard fruits with stones; Kortlandt & Kooij (1963) reported accounts of baboons using objects to crush a scorpion before eating it, to widen the entrances of ant and termite nests and to stir up insects under rocks; and van Lawick-Goodall, van Lawick & Packer (1973) observed a baboon wipe her lips with a small stone after feeding on sticky pods. Several of these manipulative performances are similar in function and execution to those which will be described under chimpanzee subsistence technology, but the levels of sophistication routinely reached by chimpanzees, especially in modification and transportation actions, are qualitatively and quantitatively beyond those achieved by baboons. For example, when considering the many thousands offield observation hours spent with baboons during the past decade or so, this list is seen to contain a mere sprinkling of incidental cases which fall rather short of establishing baboons as technologically adept Primates. This sharp limitation is in some respects anomalous, for baboon subsistence patterns are in many other ways quite closely analogous to those of omnivorous chimpanzee and human populations, especially in predatory and hunting behavior (Teleki, in press). Wild chimpanzees, on the other hand, occupy a firm position as the most technically skilled of all living nonhuman Primates, at least at the time of this writing. These
CHIMPANZEE
African lation,
apes exhibit
a rich array of technological
modification
often highly
and transportation
sophisticated
other contexts,
SUBSISTENCE
technical
behavior
of many
577
TECHNOLOGY
that incorporates
types of materials;
skills are applied in subsistence as well as in many Typical agonistic actions include shaking,
both agonistic and nonagonistic.
flailing, dragging,
throwing,
hitting and thrusting
with trees, branches,
sticks, twigs, fronds,
stones, earth clods, leaves and even food items (van Lawick-Goodall, 1968).
And nonagonistic
investigate
probing,
technical
displacement
social playing-situations and other materials
performances
1971;
(Nishida,
items,
have
behavior,
Kortlandt
(van Lawick-Goodall,
1973).
1963,
also yielded 1965,
and
leaves
in order to perform
1968)) and the Kasakati-Mahali
field experiments
valuable
information
in the use of objects
1967,
1968;
leopards
on their
as weapons
Zon & Orshoven,
exposed baboons (Eimerl
(Teleki,
in Uganda & DeVore,
1973a).
1967). between
leopard
Kortlandt,
in Guinea
potential
(Albrecht
Moreover,
to a mechanized 1965;
conducted
leopard dummies and other
not yet been observed in interactions
including
but not weapon-use
Numerous
were exposed to mechanized
and especialIy
have, however,
live carnivores,
solitary
subsistence have been recorded in Uganda (Sugiyama, 1968),
sites:
Park in Tanzania
Kortlandt,
responses
body cleaning,
sticks, twigs, vines, grasses,
in contexts other than Budongo Forest
study
and Zaire, in which chimpanzees unfamiliar
nest building,
and to fetch objects from one place to another
chimpanzee
area in Tanzania
nological
Kortlandt,
task.
Technological Gombe National
grooming,
1970;
during food and water collecting,
in which stones, branches,
distances,
some premeditated at several
skills are exhibited
are manipulated and sometimes modified to suit specific functions 1970). Ch’rm p anzees are also known to transport certain objects for
(van Lawick-Goodall, considerable
the manipu-
and their varied,
for tech& Dunnett,
Similar
‘attack’
chimpanzees
similar
and
tests in which
elicited frenzied
attacks,
pers. comm.).
This catalogue of baboon and chimpanzee technical skills shows that the frequency and distribution of such performances differ remarkably in these particular cercopithecid and pongid genera. with incidents
Reports
of chimpanzee
of technological
behavior,
only sparse cases at a very limited technical
capabilities
collector-predators, ators and human
of baboons
field research
number
of sites.
and chimpanzees
of the discrepancy
The discrepancy is reminiscent,
in the capabilities
Africa
are laden
field research
contain,
observable
even though
of chimpanzee
in the:
both are
collector-pred-,
gatherer-hunters.
3. Chimpanzee The technical
skills of chimpanzees
of subsistence,
and their technological
as well as between
throughout
whereas reports of baboon
Subsistence
are highly diversified repertoire
Technology
even within the special context
seems to contain
some flexibility
within,
populations.
(a) Skills exhibited and materials utilized The technical
skills and materials
used in naturalistic
food-getting
activities
are shown
in Table 1. When summarized, these include : pounding of hard items, such as nuts and. hard-shelled fruits, with sticks and stones; probing for honey and insects, usually ants or termites, 1OA
with twigs, vines, grass stalks and bark strips;
prying open the nests of arboreal
1
probing probing
prying (var. on probing or digging)
honey ants
ants
6
7? 8
Mt Okoro Biko
Rio Muni
Zaire Tanzania
Gombe National Park
?
termites
5
near Ayimken
termites
termites
probing
probing
probing
probing
Rio Muni
Cameroon
5
pounding pounding pounding
Manipulation
Dipikar Island
nuts palm nuts hard-shelled fruits termites
Food type
sticks, twigs
twigs sticks, twigs vines, grasses
sticks, twigs
sticks, twigs
sticks, twigs
stones stones sticks and stones sticks (twigs)
Modification
chewed and worked smooth breaking to suitable length, stripping of leaves and other projections breaking to suitable length, stripping of leaves and other projections breaking to suitable length, stripping of leaves and other projections ? breaking and biting to suitable length, stripping leaves and other projections breaking to suitable length
none( ?) none noted none noted
technology
Objects used
subsistence
Rio Muni
I? 2 3
no.
Map
data on chimpanzee
4
?
?
Site
Vital
Tai Forest Reserve regional survey
(West Africa) Liberia Ivory Coast
Country
Table
& Sabater
Pi (1969)
van Lawick-Goodall (pers. observ.)
(1970), Teleki
Merfield & Miller (1956) van Lawick-Goodall (1968, 1970)
Jones
Jones & Sabater Pi (1969)
Savage & Wyman (1843-44) Beatty (1951) Rahm (1971), Struhsaker & Hunkeler (197i) ‘Struhsaker & Hunkeler (1971) Jones & Sabater Pi (1969)
References
Tanzania
Makolongo & Kabogo Pt Mahali Mtns
Tanzania
Basin
Kasakati
Tanzania
prying (var. on probing or digging) wiping (var. on sponging)
ants
ants
probing
probing probing
honey termites ants
probing
termites
ants
sponging
water
leaves
sticks, twigs vines, bark strips sticks
twigs twigs, vines
twigs, bark strips twigs, bark strips
leaves
sticks, twigs leaves
probing sponging
honey brain tissue
sticks twigs, vines grasses
digging
termites
ants
crumpled by hand into wad
none noted
breaking and biting to suitable length, stripping of leaves and projections
breaking to suitable length, stripping of leaves and other projections none noted none noted
breaking to suitable length, stripping of leaves and other projections none noted chewed into an absorbent mass chewed into an absorbent mass breaking to suitable length
breaking to suitable length
Nishida (1973)
Nishida (1973)
Nishida (1973)
Izawa & Itani (1966) Izawa & Itani (1966)
Suzuki (1969)
Suzuki (1966, 1969)
van Lawick-Goodall
van Lawick-Goodall Teleki (19738)
van Lawick-Goodall (pers. observ.) van Lawick-Goodall (pers. observ.)
(1968,
( 1970)
1970)
( 1968, 1970)) Teleki
(1970), Teleki
580
G. TELEKI
ants with sticks;
poking at unfamiliar
‘olfactory
digging into the terrestrial
aids’;
items, including
foods, with sticks or grasses used as
nests of safari ants with sticks;
from hollows in trees, and brain tissue from a skull cavity, by chewing;
and z.@ing up ants with leaves crumpled
An interesting observed ation
footnote
to use objects
have been recorded
technical
skills and predatory
among baboons;
habits
technology
the actions
skills.
then
in the application
absent
the opening
which
is somewhat
of technological
performed
comprise
as variations
chimpanzee
probing,
analogous
and human
chimpanzee
and
technique
subsistence
If prying,
themes of digging,
It is noteworthy,
limitation
poking
probing
as distinct
and
technical
among human gatherer-hunters,
but it occurs rarely,
of some gatherer-hunter
of objects
involve several types of foods in variety.
probing
having
been seen only
however,
that an activity
for insects-is
widely
But the tubers and other subsurface
Herein lies another
of modification,
to
skills to subsistence
a rift between
digging and sponging remain
to digging-namely,
by chimpanzees.
apes.
The manipulation by actions
of technical
limited
on the respective
of ant nests with sticks.
the staple resources
these omnivorous
of a link between
is on the one hand similar
behavior
are themselves
from this repertoire;
performed
183 cases of pred-
devices.
only pounding,
during
frequently
The absence
chimpanzees
Digging with sticks, which is a standard
is not entirely
in press).
by virtue of the fact that the latter hunt almost exclusively
the above examples
and wiping are regarded sponging,
among
observable
with the aid of technological and objects,
1960 (Teleki,
have not yet been
prey, even though
but on the other it serves to emphasize
subsistence
Although
or dismembering
since
other kinds of limitations human
by hand.
to the above list is the fact that chimpanzees for killing
sponging water
with wads of leaves crumpled
and
flora which
populations
cannot be exploited
highlighting
the difference
by
between
technology.
in contexts
other than subsistence
so it is doubly
significant
that
is rarely
many
accompanied
of the food-getting
technical skills exhibited by chimpanzees include a component ofpreparatory modification. The form of an object is most commonly modified when materials are prepared for probing and sponging:
twigs and vines are stripped
vines and grasses are broken
or bitten
of leaves and other projections;
off to suitable
lengths;
sticks, twigs,
and leaves are chewed
or
manually crumpled into absorbent masses. These observations suggest that there may be, and may well have been, a stronger linkage between modification and subsistence activities
than between
modification
and other patterns
of technological
behavior,
such
as aggressive use of objects. Chimpanzees sometimes carry objects about for periods of more than an hour and for Such transport behavior occurs mainly in connection distances greater than a kilometer. with
termite
mound
collecting,
as an individual
when
an appropriate
seeks new tunnels.
probe
Materials
may
be carried
from
mound
to
such as vines and grass stems are
provides usually carried in the hands or between the lips. The context of insect-collecting the only definite cases where transportation is an intermediate activity between a series of applications. But there are other situations in which transportation is an anticipatory activity, as when a chimpanzee sets off to probe for termites and then selects and prepares an appropriate vine or grass stalk some 100 or more meters before arriving at a mound. And in still other situations chimpanzees will leave one place to fetch an object from another : e.g. when getting a new probe during a ‘fishing’ session, or when getting an The apparent absence of transport behavior object to strike or throw at an antagonist. in baboons, the conditional presence of rudimentary transport behavior in chimpanzees,
CHIMPANZEE
and the routine of ordered
practice
Surveys have
of long-distance
levels of complexity of Primate
used in agonistic been
stimulus,
behavior,
as proposed by Kortlandt
of objects
probably
Similarly, Primate
whereas
the emergence
& Kooij
in humans
are most commonly
behavioral
context
for developing
was initially
(1963))
the modification
with subsistence
skills probably
of pongids.
appeared
and transportation
with
Whereas
in the very early
in technology,
An evolutionary feedback
much
perhaps
model of technological
system wherein
applied to weapon gathering
later,
with the emergence
development
modification
stages
of
until
such as a shift from
of the earliest
flora, may hominids.
would, in this case, have involved
and probably
long-distance
making long after these activities
subsistence
sticks
for use as probes.
skills may not have appeared
More subtle refinements
skills.
agonistic
and transportation
activities.
the use of sticks to expose insect nests to the use of sticks to dig for subsurface only have appeared
and widely
may, in effect, technical
associated
for use as clubs, twigs must usually be modified
modification
are suggestive
technology.
source,
of objects
in conjunction
manipulative
evolution,
This
or original
even if the manipulation
need not be modified
behavior
subsistence
1963).
581
TECHNOLOGY
skills show that objects
(e.g. Hall,
emerged
transport
in Primate
technical
situations
the Primary
However,
SUBSISTENCE
became
transportation
functional
a
were
in a collecting
or
and ecology have been conducted
in
context.
(b) Geografihical and individual variability The most intensive eastern other
Africa, parts
subsistence
of the technology
The repertoire area
studies of chimpanzee
but evidence
(including
continent.
The
Basin,
For example,
at the westernmost
has recently
geographical
Makolongo,
Kabogo
But information
been discovered
distribution
1, which supplements National
in
of chimpanzee
the data listed in Table
1,
Park and in the Kasakati-Mahali Point,
Mahali
from other regions,
Mtns)
is probably
and notably
from
portion of the Pan range, is likely to be rather incomplete.
most of the pounding
data from Cameroon
behavior
in Gombe
close to being fully documented. populations
known
is shown in Figure
of skills performed Kasakati
behavior
of technological
and Rio Muni,
data from Liberia
and Ivory Coast, and the probing
derive from the material
Figure 1. Geographical complexes.
distribution
remains of
of these activities.
chimpanzee
technological
582
G. TELEKI
Hence, an assessment of any geographical variability that may occur in technological traits rests on much firmer ground at the eastern than at the western extremes of the continent. Long-term studies in western and central Africa are sorely needed; until such have been conducted, the following regional comparisons remain highly tentative, being more an instructive exercise than a final statement of fact. The close similarities observed in the technical skills exhibited at adjacent sites, such as those in Tanzania or in Cameroon and Rio Muni, attests to the behavioral integrity of what probably were, until quite recently, larger population units. Based on this assumption, the two neighboring sites in Liberia and Ivory Coast (2 and 3 on map), the three intermediate sites in Cameroon and Rio Muni (4, 5 and 6), and the two adjacent sites in Tanzania (8 and 9) can be treated as three discrete units labeled as populations A, B and C, respectively. Table 1 showed that pounding is associated with hard fruits and nuts, probing with insects and honey, digging with ants, and sponging with water and brain tissue. These all appear to be highly functional relationships in which the actions performed and the materials utilized are closely tied to the types and locations of the resources being exploited. These functional relationships define technological complexes which consist of specific linkages in patterned actions, in materials and in foods (e.g. pounding on nuts with stones), and these complexes in turn combine to form the technological repertoire of a given population. The patterned actions may themselves be composed of behavioral sequences, such as scanning, locating, selecting, picking up, shaping and utilizing, all of which are preliminary to food consumption. Furthermore, each complex contains little or no variability from region to region-that is, the pattern of probing for termites is similarly performed wherever the complex appears-but the matrix of complexes which comprises a full repertoire may show considerable variability from one region to another. The full complement of technical skills-pounding, probing, digging and sponging-has not been documented at any single study site, nor in any one of the three labeled populations. Pounding is currently known only in population A. Despite 19 months of field surveying in the Cameroon area of population B, Struhsaker found no evidence of this trait (Struhsaker & Hunkeler, 1971); similarly, 20 months of chimpanzee surveys in the Rio Muni area of population B yielded no signs of pounding (Jones & Sabater Pi, 1971). In population C, hard-shelled fruits are opened simply by hitting the items themselves against hard surfaces, such as rocks or tree trunks (Izawa & Itani, 1966; van LawickGoodall, 1968). The pounding technique, in which one object is used to hit another, thus seems to be restricted to regions west of a zone that is approximately delineated by 5’E longitude. Probing for insects and honey has been extensively recorded in the regions inhabited by populations B and C, and this is the most widespread complex known today. Some intrapopulation variability may occur in this complex: both populations B and C probe for termites whereas only population C is known to probe for ants; and members of population B seem to favor sticks and twigs while members of population C seem to prefer grass stalks, vines and bark strips. In broad terms, the zone marked by the 5’E longitude line serves as the approximate western limit for the probing technique, as no evidence of this complex has been obtained in population A (Struhsaker & Hunkeler, 1971). Sponging up water and brain tissue, or wiping up ants, is known at present only in population C. Although the sponging complex may be a regional variation on the same scale as the pounding complex, there is some possibility that this technical skill has not
CHIMPANZEE
SUBSISTENCE
TECHNOLOGY
583
yet been observed outside Tanzania simply because of differences in the scope of field studies conducted elsewhere. The distribution of digging, which has also been observed only in Tanzania, may be similarly biased. However, when the facts are taken as they now stand, pounding is restricted to population A, sponging and digging to population C, and probing to populations B and C. The longitudinal lines of 5”E and 25”E serve as rough boundaries for the technological repertoires. It is perhaps noteworthy that the 29”E meridian, which separates the main basins of central and western Africa from thle rift zone of eastern Africa, has been cited as a rough boundary for baboon and chimpanzese predation on mammals, a phenomenon which occurs at several sites east of this lin’e (Kortlandt & Kooij, 1963; Teleki, in press). Not even the most widely exhibited technical skill, probing for insects, has been recorded at some sites lying well within the range of this complex. In Uganda, for example:, where investigators have intermittently studied chimpanzees since 1962 (Reynolds & Reynolds, 1965 ; Sugiyama, 1968, 1969), all forms of subsistence technology seem to be absent. Such negative evidence is at best dubiously reliable, but the apparent absence of certain complexes in local populations does introduce the issue of technological variability within a regional population. There is some evidence to indicate that the patterns of technological variation outlined above are less a function of opportunistic than of behavioral variability. Although termites are known to occur from the westernmost to the easternmost chimpanzee habitats where some sort of subsistence technology is practiced, these insects are not everywhere exploited via the probing technique. Similarly, the nuts of oil palm trees are apparently consumed by chimpanzees throughout Africa, yet the pounding technique has been observed only in the westernmost region. These same nuts are a favored food of population C as well, but the latter simply strip away the external pulp with their teeth and sometimes swallow the nuts whole. If further field studies show that technological complexes do vary within or between populations despite uniformly available food resources, an element of cultural heritage may play a part in the transmission of skills. The acquistion and propagation of a technical skill, such as probing for termites, is known to involve learning processes which are in turn probably affected by individua.1 differences in ability. Chimpanzee infants learn their ‘fishing’ skills by watching older individuals and then practicing the technique for several years (van Lawick-Goodall, 1968, 1970), and the level of sophistication achieved by the time adulthood is reached is noIt necessarily uniform in all members of the community, or social unit. These individual differences may lead to considerable variability in the performance of any given technical skill. Thus, the transmission of such a tradition might be dependent upon both the sophistication of the performants and the aptitude of the learners. Such individual variability is in fact observable in many contexts other than that of subsistence technology. Some members of a chimpanzee community participate regularly in predatory episodes whereas others may avoid these situations entirely (Teleki, 19733) ; some members of a group may consistently use objects in attacking a mechanized leopard whereas others exhibit no such technical skills (Kortlandt, pers. comm.). The examples are many and diverse, individual variability being a pervasive theme in most ofthe activities performed by chimpanzees wherever they have been studied in depth. Technological behavior may, therefore, be but one of many activities that are differentially performed within a single population and perhaps even within a single social unit.
584
G. TELEKI
The mechanisms whereby chimpanzee technological repertoires differentiate through time remain obscure largely because the data are still so incomplete. Models of behavioral variability in subsistence habits must therefore be sought elsewhere. Intensive studies of certain Japanese macaque populations, in which kin, consort and other social ties regulate transmission of such spontaneously acquired subsistence techniques as potatowashing and wheat-sifting (e.g. Azuma, 1973), provide one potential model. The similar social mechanisms which have been tentatively identified as regulators of chimpanzee predatory behavior (Teleki, 19736) constitute another potential model. In both of these situations, not all members of a social unit learn and apply the various subsistence techniques uniformly and unilaterally. Depending on the extent of inter-unit migration, such variability could enhance or inhibit transmission of a given behavior within and/or between populations. Most Primates can be classed as omnivores, but the degree to which any one population or social unit is omnivorous can vary tremendously. Baboon and chimpanzee populations, for example, exhibit great variability in predatory frequency, perhaps because this behavior is affected by both social and environmental contingencies (Teleki, in press). So the mechanisms regulating a subsistence activity, such as predatory behavior, and any other activity associated with subsistence, such as technological behavior, may set the stage for differential development of skills. Physical and/or psychological limitations on acquiring and transmitting technical skills may, on the other hand, offset this variability in such a way that an upper limit to technological achievement is met at some point. The balance between exploitative skills and available resources would, in effect, tend to remain at equilibrium at a given place and over a short period of time. Consequently, the Gilgil baboons (e.g. Harding, 1973) can no more be expected to exhibit the kinds of technical skills (and predatory habits) possessed by the Gombe chimpanzees (e.g. Teleki, 1973c) than can the latter be expected to exhibit the kinds of technical skills (and hunting habits) possessed by the Kalahari bushmen (e.g. Silberbauer, 1972)) even though all three populations are fully omnivorous. 4. Chimpanzee Inquiry
Termiting
Skills:
A Personal
The techniques used to probe into termite mounds and ant nests have been closely studied at the Gombe and Mahali sites (van Lawick-Goodall, 1968, 1970; Nishida, 1973). My purpose here is to supplement these descriptions with comments on the expertise needed by chimpanzees to perform this task successfully. During the course of two termite-collecting seasons, in 1968 and 1970, I followed Gombe chimpanzees on their rounds of termite mounds with the intention of learning about the following problems. (1) How does a chimpanzee find the entrances to termite tunnels which, during the diurnal foraging period of these apes, are covered by an homogeneous layer of clay? (2) How does a chimpanzee select the object to be used in probing for termites, and what are the essential properties which enable the selected object to function suitably ? And (3) what actions must be executed in order to ensure a rewarding catch of termites ? Early in my observation I had become impressed by the ease and facility with which adult chimpanzees performed the probing technique, and my queries were prompted by a suspicion that the existing descriptions of this technological complex, which seemed to focus mainly on the mechanical actions involved, did
Plate 1. Adult male chimpanzee (Faben) carefully draws a length of grass from a termite tunnel with his left hand and begins to extend his lower lip in preparation to pulling the soldier termites off the ‘fishing’ tool.
584
Plate Z.(a) Adult male chimpanzee (Leakey, on left) holds a probing tool between his lips while scraping the mound surface with a forefinger in order to expose a tunnel mouth. His estrous female (Fifi) consort watches intently. (b) Adult male chimpanzee (Figan) strips soldier termites from a grass probing tool, using his lips and teeth to remove the termites while stabilizing the tool on his wrist.
Plate 3.(a) Infant female chimpanzee (Porn) attempts to fish for termites at a tunnel just abandoned by her mother; however, the tool she uses is inappropriately stiff and thick and will not enter the tunnel (note that the grass stem is beginning to buckle). (b) Adult male chimpanzee (Figan) walks between two termite mounds, in this case more than a hundred meters apart, carrying his probing stick in his mouth (note that stick is frayed at the end, where it was broken to a suitable length and that it has been stripped of leaves).
Plate 4.(a) Juvenile male chimpanzee (Sniff) twirls a branch about with both feet in solitary play, also exhibiting a moderately intense play face. (b) Adult female chimpanzee (Flo, upper left) intently grooms a handful of leaves while two of her offspring (Flint, left foreground; Fifi, upper right) watch the lip and finger movements closely. Such ‘objectgrooming’ often serves to elicit social grooming from those who watched the performer.
CHIMPANZEE
not adequately
convey what chimpanzees
In particular,
insects.
I wished
requisites involved. One old and seemingly during
the second habits,
important
which
with regular
routinely
executed
to imitate
of Leakey,
the physical
activity,
which insight eventually pongid technical
pre-
a study of ranging
habits,
it seemed evident by then that patterned visits to certain
mounds,
efficiency
the techniques
might
under observation, experience
the ‘uniqueness’
the final
revealed
new
in this subsistence
led to the suspicion that anthropologists
skills when defining
while alone
including
involved
an
More-
in the skills so
below, these experiences factors
play
and competence.
(All aspects of the procedure, As described
and psychological
estimate
the psychological
was followed most consistently
so as to gain personal
by this individual.
about
about
Leakey,
of an individual’s
tasting of the insects, were practised.) information
in. order to ‘fish’ skillfully for these
time I was conducting
versus random
endeavored
or when in the company
585
TECHNOLOGY
something
structure-because
part in the development
over, I frequently
must k&g
to discover
highly skilled individual,
season-at
daily regimen and community ranging
SUBSISTENCE
tend to under-
of man.
(a) Setting the stage As reported
by van LawickGoodall(1968,
the following tunnel,
basic actions:
using a finger
scraping
1970), termite-probing,
or thumb;
mouth; pausing momentarily while holding the protruding extracting the object with its attached soldier and worker desired termites
(soldiers being preferred)
in one hand and stabilizing Gombe
study population
with the lips and teeth while holding the probe
activity
Termite-probing onset of rainfall reproductive the rains
is an annual
2 months
per year,
in October
when termites
and migratory
begin.
There
activity, period
evidence
cues, as they sometimes
rains.
Out-of-season
termiting
rarely
practiced
during
regularly
at Gombe
these mounds,
presumably
in other circumstances (b) Locating
tend
of all but
to show the
as the ‘fishing’ season only being
occurring
chimpanzees
closely linked
begin to examine
mounds
these
to their mounds. because
it contains
(except
but the probing
technique
It is also relevant
during the migration
Chimpanzees
sometimes
salt, a mineral
seasons,
prior to the first
and opened even during the most intense collection nocturnal
to the
the termite
about 2-3 weeks after
anticipate
of the dry or rainy seasons.
are essentially
block all entrances
that
Individuals
adults
shift also regulates
bouts do occur occasionally,
the height
tunnels must daily be relocated for termites
This climatic
cycles, with mass migration
perhaps via climatic
4-5 hr
are in season.
but not an everyday
is some
of the
hr and sometimes
skills, although
the most productive
or November.
Nearly all members
devote at least l-2
the youngest age classes exhibit the probing greatest persistence and proficiency. lasts about
end of the object in one hand; termites; and picking off the
the free end on the other wrist.
(ca. 50 individuals)
daily to this one subsistence
or ‘fishing’, consists of
away a thin soil layer that covers the entrance of a carefully poking an object into the exposed tunnel
which
is
that
period,
time)
and
suck on soil from they actively
seek
as well.
tunnels
Having repeatedly observed individuals approach a mound, make a rapid visual scan of the surface while standing on or beside it, and reach decisively out-with a high degree of predictive accuracy-to uncover a tunnel, I was soon impressed by the apparent
586
0.
TELEKI
ease with which tunnels could be located. In attempting to learn the technique, I applied several experimental procedures : examining in minute detail all crack patterns, protuberances, depressions and other “topographic” features in the clay. But, after weeks of futile searching for the essential clue, I had to resort to scraping mound surfaces with a jackknife until a tunnel was inadvertently exposed. My inability to find any physical features which could serve as visual cues eventually led me to realize that chimpanzees may possess knowledge far beyond my expectations. Consistent following of chimpanzees during the two termiting seasons, and especially among the mounds scattered throughout lower Kakombe Valley (see Teleki, 19736, for maps of the study area), revealed that some individuals routinely visit certain sets of mounds, often travelling along a prescribed network of trails from one to another in the sequence. Thus, each chimpanzee seemed to exploit a core set of mounds most heavily, although other mounds encountered during more extensive forays are of course utilized on an opportunistic basis. Presumably the mounds in the core set, which may vary in number from about 10 to 25 per individual, are more familiar to a given individual than are the mounds less regularly visited. Some or all of the core mounds may be visited on a daily basis during the collecting season, but the regularity of visits may be influenced by other behavioral opportunities (e.g. an estrous female) which may be present at the time. Many of the mounds within a core set may be shared by several individuals, so the ‘termiting ranges’ may overlap extensively. Because only 1-3 chimpanzees are likely to work a mound simultaneously, competition occurs only rarely. There is some indication, however, that adult females, and particularly mothers, who tend to have much smaller ranges than other adult classes, exhibit an unusual degree of competition for this resource (Baldwin, pers. comm.). A chimpanzee’s presumed familiarity with a core set of mounds provides the main clue to the problem of locating tunnels. The only hypothesis which, at this point, seems to reasonably account for the observed facts is that an adult chimpanzee may know (memorize?) the precise location of 100 or more tunnels in the most familiar mounds. Moreover, since intensive probing is restricted to a short annual season, the possibility that chimpanzees retain a mental map of core mound features during the intervening 10 months must also be considered. That chimpanzees require a prolonged learning period (i.e. 4-5 years) to gain proficiency in this technique (van Lawick-Goodall, 1970), and that some individuals are known to have the capability to retain specific information for many years, provides circumstantial support for this hypothesis. However, only a detailed study of termite-collecting habits, and of termite behavior, is likely to resolve the following kinds of questions. Is the error rate in locating tunnels higher at mounds that are less familiar than those in a core set ? Are the tunnels used by a given individual in a core set of mounds consistently the same for long periods of time? Is there a period of greater error in locating tunnels at the start of each season? (c) Selecting materials
An examination of the skills needed for selecting an object having the requisite properties for successful probing was the next step in the investigation. When performed by experienced chimpanzees, the selection procedure seems deceptively simple. After a brief visual scan of the nearby vegetation, a chimpanzee will usually extend a hand and defty tear off a twig, vine or grass stalk. Sometimes the individual must move a few paces away
CHIMPANZEE
from the mound selected.
These
and fetch a suitable may be rapidly
SUBSISTENCE
probe,
examined
in one, or several may be carried
587
TECHNOLOGY
and in some cases 2-3
and discarded
to the mound
objects
are initially
until some specification
for subsequent
selection.
is met
Whenever
it
occurs, the selection is made in a swift, almost casual manner, and modification is begun if necessary. Without being aware of the nuances involved, it is easy to undervalue the proficiency needed to perform these manouvers. Chimpanzees presumably have the experience can be evaluated
probes is not high.
For the novice human
sequence
holds the key to selection
apparent
only through
observer,
because
the trial-and-error
dimensions, or biting
before
motor patterns
are other physical
which
of an object
the utilization
phase of the
of an object
It is not difficult
becomes to find a
nor is it difficult to trim away leaves and
it down to the required
which entail some dexterity
properties
less easy to anticipate. stringent:
breaking
however,
of application.
other
projections
the properties
for the rate of error in selecting
the appropriateness
grass stalk or vine stem of suitable routine
whereby
before it is applied to the task of probing,
must precisely
When probing for termites,
length.
These
but little prior experience. fit the intended
the specifications
are
But there
task, and these are
are in fact surprisingly
if the vine or grass selected is too pliant, it will buckIe and collapse (accordion-, if, on the other hand, the object is too stiff or
like) when inserted into a twisting tunnel; brittle,
it will catch on the tunnel walls and either break or resist entry to the necessary
depth.
An intermediate
range
yield many termites. Despite months of observing enviable Similar
of qualities
and aping adult chimpanzees
ease, speed and accuracy, ineptness
My experiences locating, retention
suggest that selection proficiency
entail a considerable of the results.
ants from terrestrial of the
I was unable
to achieve
can only be observed in chimpanzees
individual. Behind these refinements nature
must therefore
The
amount
span
resources
being
exploited.
as they selected probes with1 their level of competence,.
may
behavior
actually
collecting foremost,
ants are large,
up with a stick.
stiff sticks or branches
into the nest without
bending
by long-term
the lifetime
used in collecting
of an
termites
and
in their properties, due in large part to the At Gombe, termite mounds (Macroterme:r
ant nests (Anomma nigricans) consist of loosely aggregated away by hand or stirred
backed
cover
belbicosus) are covered by a uniform clay Iayer which is extremely pushed
is to1
is a learned skill which must, like tunnel-
is the fact that the materials
nests differ considerably
if probing
below the age of about 4-5 years.
of trial-and-error
retention
be selected
The which
or breaking.
hard when dry, whereas
soil particles objects
which can easily be
commonly
can be forcefully
The dimensions
selected shoved,
and properties
for end of
these objects are thus quite different from materials used in probing, where the combined criteria of pliability and tensile strength are more important. To complicate the issue further,
the techniques
and materials
used to collect ants from arboreal
nests (Crematogaste:r
spp. and Camponotus spp.), as described by van Lawick-Goodall (1968) and Nishida (1973)) resemble those used in collecting termites from terrestrial mounds. Exploitation of each food resource
therefore
entails selection
procedures
geared to a specific task and resource.
Given the possibility that technological repertoires differ from region to region, each stage of a particular complex may well have to be learned via a cultural tradition that is passed from individual to individual within a given population, one of the most direct and stable routes offspring.
of learning
being
the prolonged
associations
between
mothers
and their
588
G.
TELEKI
(d) Utilizing probes Having
failed to meet chimpanzee
of termite-probing, stage,
which
probes. weeks
seemed
relatively
Consequently,
interval, particular
of aptitude
set out to acquire
simple
them
total
out again-without
failure-an
panthropologist-did tunnel
to locating probes,
getting
uncommonly
learning
(in which
termites
and selecting
for the designated
any termites.
brief
stages
in the ‘fishing’
tunnels
pausing
Only
span,
I finally begin to grasp the problems
a very rich tunnel may initially produce moderate a newly opened
and skill in the preceding
some competence
in comparison
I spent many hours inserting
and pulling of nearly
standards
I nonetheless
after some
at least
involved.
for this Although
rewards even if skill is essentially lacking,
are perhaps
still too far underground)
or a
partly ‘fished-out’ the application ful probing
tunnel (which termites may block off at some subsurface level) requires Skill is particularly necessary if the period of successof subtle techniques.
is to last more than a few minutes.
In order to collect fully and dextrously
these subterranian inserted
wrist so that the object
termites,
the probing
to a depth of about 8-16
navigates
the twisting
object
must first be care-
cm, with appropriate
channel.
The probe
turns of the
must then be gently
vibrated with the fingers during the prescribed pause, for without this movement the termites may not be stimulated into biting firmly onto the probe. However, if the vibration is performed
too lengthily
be cut through have
been
attached,
or roughly,
by the mandibles
correctly
performed,
must be extracted
If the object
is too rapidly
there is an excellent
while still in the tunnel. the probe,
chance
presumably
with
dozens
or clumsily
which then yields nothing
but not overly swift and, once started,
graceful.
If the tunnel is particularly of the probe),
of termites
now
Once again there are nuances to be observed. pulled out, the insects are likely to be scraped off
motions insertion
will
actions
from the tunnel.
along the sides of the tunnel, must be reasonably
that the probe
When these preliminary
tortuous
but a shredded
probe.
uniformly
The hand fluid and
(a feature which can be determined
during
the success of the catch can be ensured by a slow twisting
of the
wrist while the probe is pulled out. (e) Considering the results These,
then,
are the sequential
behavioral
components
of a chimpanzee
technical
skill
which, when viewed superficially, seems to be a far simpler task than those routinely performed by human gatherer-hunters. But my participant observation at Gombe unveiled
the remarkable amount of experience and finesse needed by a chimpanzee to perform this subsistence activity. When such qualitative aspects of subsistence technology are omitted from descriptions, the risk of dispensing with such skills as ‘rudimentary’ increases. Incompetent as they were, my attempts to acquire the skills of locating termites left me with a tunnels, selecting materials, inserting probes, and extracting healthy measure of respect for chimpanzee technical ability, as well as with a nagging suspicion that the physical and psychological capabilities needed to develop, apply and transmit such skills may differ in degree but not in kind from those needed by humans to locate, expose and gather insects and subsurface flora. In other words, a question about how an integrated sequence of technological behavior, where the omission of any single stage results in failure, was initiated, put together and finally fixed into the repertoire of a population, can now be directed at nonhuman as well as human Primates.
CHIMPANZEE
If these tentative gap postulated
conclusions
are confirmed
to exist between
though not entirely
SUBSISTENCE
by further field research,
the nonhuman
and human
The key word here is ‘partly’,
filled.
589
TECHNOLOGY
the technological
Primates
would be partly
for it remains
clear that the full
scope of subsistence technology probably differs considerably, in terms of both the range human gatherer-hunters and the refinement of skills, from one Primate genus to another: are unquestionably
more
than are nonhuman
dependent
on a wider array
5. Termiting The simplest way to illustrate data
on the termiting
Africa.
This
sample
exhibited
of Primate
Interspecific
skills and materials
phylogenetic
some comparative:
chimpanzees
and humans
from a cercopithecid
to mirror the increasingly
different
Comparisons
in degree is by reviewing by baboons,
populations-ranging
as well as the levels at which limitations having
Skills :
such differences
techniques
pongid to a hominid-serves primates
of technical
collector-predators.
compIex technological
arise, that can be observed
in.
through
a.
capabilities,.
in the behavior
of
histories.
(u) Baboon and chimpanzee techniques General
differences
already
in the technological
been summarized.
termite-collecting National
Park,
skills of these (1968),
baboons,
habits
The
baboons
simple
and flying insects are collected
be seen leaping methods
(These
colobus
as they emerge,
do two or more
object
Several
collect
the apes collect
as a probe,
period,
baboons
insects.
mouth.
especially
But observers
also collect
instead probe for soldiers elsewhere chimpanzee
technique.
limited
to the 2-3
simultaneously, are sometimes
by adult males, and may then using an
on a mound or abandoned termites
the insects massing
on the same mound.
As a result, the termite-collecting
weeks before and after the short migration
Baboons
have not seen baboons
some winged
in
such as the ‘leaf-eating’
may work a mound
at a single tunnel
yet they will in many cases ignore
additional
of
mass and fly away from open
Primates,
even when these have been discarded
tunnel by the chimpanzees. The Gombe chimpanzees
and
to focus on modes
up and down on mounds in an effort to grab termites
displaced from mounds by arriving chimpanzees, sit and watch
of Gombe (1971)
with hands and/or lips, and baboons
are also used by other Gombe
and the blue monkey.)
but rarely
opportunity
by Ransom
season is almost exclusively
singly or in clusters,
can sometimes
and chimpanzees
have: in the
Thus, their bulk intake consists of winged termites
occur.
tunnels. flight.
differences
in a single habitat.
that are collected, Crawling
and chimpanzees specific
have been described
a uniquely
the termiting
weeks during which migrations
of baboons
here is to highlight
Primates.
provide
a single food resource
For the Gombe
behavior
purpose
whose insect-eating
Lawick-Goodall exploiting
The
during at tunnel
in a
the migration entrances
and
Probing is clearly the standard season is extended by several
period,
and the bulk intake consists
of soldier termites. The technical exploitation of this food resource, differ markedly
skills developed by chimpanzees have clearly affected and the exploitative potentials of the two genera now in duration if notin bulk. And, although both primates show a great fancy
for termites, their respective limited-supply resource.
preferences
and methods
largely
obviate
competition
for a
590
G.
TELEKI
The value of the Gombe example lies in its exposure of what appear to be fixed patterns of behavior in baboons and to some extent in chimpanzees. What, in other words, inhibits baboons from adopting, at least to a limited degree, the techniques used by chimpanzees ? And what kinds of psychological phenomena are involved in this limitation? The questions become doubly intriguing when set against a larger regional background, for this apparent limitation is not necessarily characteristic of the genus Pa&o. According to the incidental reports collected by Kortlandt & Kooij (1963), some baboons do use pieces of wood to widen the entrances of termite nests. Whether this is a regional difference in behavior comparable to those documented for chimpanzee populations A, B and C remains unknown, as does the problem of whether the more technically skilled baboons exhibit similar levels of sophistication and persistence as do chimpanzees. Further field studies are much needed to elucidate such problems. Given the general differences between baboon and chimpanzee technological repertoires, as well as the specific discrepancies observed in termite-collecting at Gombe, it is currently tempting to postulate that the technical skills in these genera are related to varying levels of cortical complexity, and hence to differential learning and performing capacities. If so, a model of behavioral evolution might be integrated with the numerous existing models of biological evolution. But there are crucial, as yet unresolved issues: for example, to what extent are such technological limitations characteristic of species or genera, and how are the limitations adaptively significant? (b) Chimpanzee and human techniques
In a survey of human insect-eating habits, Bodenheimer (195 1) shows that termites, ants and other insects are highly favored by numerous human populations, many of which live in regions also inhabited by baboons and/or chimpanzees. Taking Africa in its entirety, human techniques of termite-gathering are extremely variable and often much more efficient than those of termite-collecting baboons and chimpanzees; on a local level, however, some of the human techniques are the same as those used by nonhuman Primates. In western Zaire, for example, members of the Baluba tribe insert palm leaves into termite tunnels, pause to stir the protruding probes, and finally withdraw the probes laden with clinging soldier termites. Elsewhere in western Zaire, large poles are driven into termite mounds to make holes about a meter in depth; the poles are withdrawn, and small “brooms” made of grass are inserted into these holes ; a noise is then made with the tongue so as to imitate the sound of raindrops falling on the clay, and the probes are turned slightly by hand so that termites will clamp onto the grass; then the probes are withdrawn, and the soldier termites are eaten one-by-one immediately, or they may be collected in baskets and taken away. A variation on the theme of imitating raindrops occurs in northern Zaire, where two pieces of wood are beaten against the mound surface. A rather common form of suberfuge, stick-beating has also been observed at Mt Elgon, on the UgandaKenya border, and other areas. The soil of termite mounds is in some places also eaten, the claim being that such soil contains higher concentrations of salt. At Gombe, where termites are also actively sought by the indigenous human population, individuals or small groups frequently go termite-gathering during the rainy season. The insects are often gathered at dusk, the following technique being commonly used: a kerosene lantern is placed on or near the mound, both to aid vision and to act as
CHIMPANZEE
SUBSISTENCE
TECHNOLOGY
591
a beacon for the insects; a large portion of the mound surface is scraped away with a panga (machette) blade to expose and stir up the insects; the exposed milling mass of termites is picked or scooped up into a dish or basket by hand and sometimes by leaves or fronds. During the 2-3 week migration season mainly winged termites are gathered, but both soldiers and workers are taken during the rest of the rainy season. On one occasion I observed a man rapping on a mound with two small sticks, presumably in order to imitate raindrops. Thus, three ways of exploiting a given resource, two of which involve technological behavior, can be compared at this one site. The human technique further extends the duration of the termite-gathering season, much beyond the periods in which baboons and even chimpanzees can collect these insects, and perhaps increases the annual yield as well. The baboon, chimpanzee and human populations in Gombe National Park clearly exhibit a series of increasingly sophisticated technical skills which reflect distinct, perhaps species-specific, levels of technological (and psychological?) capabilities. After emphasizing the remarkable degree of similarity which occurs in some chimpanzee and human termiting techniques, it should also be stressed that the above comparisons are not fully representative of the rich array of skills performed by various human populations. Whereas chimpanzees throughout much of Africa possess a single technological complex for termite-collecting, human populations exhibit extensive regional variability in techniques which can include fumigating, digging, pouring water onto mounds, placing upturned dishes onto sticks driven vertically into the clay, and so on. Moreover, only humans are known to practice the subtle subterfuge of imitating raindrops so as to entice termites to the surface. This one skill may alone extend the gathering season by weeks or months. These anecdotal cases do not provide a comprehensive picture of Primate technical skills. Numerous gaps must be filled in with further comparative field work. But the examples do suggest that subsistence activities, and to some extent even technical skills, can be arranged as overlapping segments on a behavioral continuum. When such continuities can be documented among extant cercopithecid, pongid and hominid populations, the resultant behavioral models can be used to supplement morphological trends in primate evolution and also to lay a foundation for reconstructing the probable subsistence habits of the earliest hominids. Although attention has focused here on technological behavior, this approach can be adopted with other subsistence activities, such as predatory and hunting behavior (Teleki, in press). 6. Conclusion
and Discussion
The application of technical skills to subsistence activities, and particularly to hunting game, has been widely cited as a characteristically human condition. The material remains of technological behavior, such as shaped artefacts, are often regarded by prehistorians as a sign of human cuhure, and have thus been ranked as a prime ingredient in the process of hominization (e.g. Oakley, 1959). This conceptual trend was readily adopted in other branches of the discipline, and notably by those cultural anthropologists who were interested in separating nonhuman primate society and primitive human culture (e.g. Sahlins, 1959). Now deeply ingrained in our minds and our texts, the trend continues to be propagated in these circles (e.g. Fried, 1967) despite the many advances which have begun to sway members of other subdisciplines toward an alternative position (e.g. Lancaster, 1968; Mann, 1972). Needless to say, the impact of seperatism upon
592
G. TELEKI
anthropological theories of human evolution has been immense, for the above is by no means an isolated case history. Many traits other than technology-among which can be numbered predatory behavior, nuclear family structure, division of labor by sex, kinship relationships and lineage ties, incest prohibitions, sharing and cooperation-have been routinely linked to the divergence of a savanna-dwelling, meat-hunting hominid from a forest-dwelling, plant-collecting Primate heritage. That this determination to regard humans as behaviorally and psychologically supreme (an attitude which retains a strong flavor of “special creation”) contrasts with a readiness to accept biological affinities (an attitude which often leads to scientific destruction in the name of human welfare) does not seem to have caused excessive consternation among students of human behavior and evolution. Yet it steadily becomes more obvious, as evidence accumulates about what Primates actually do in their native habitats, that nearly all of these ‘human’ traits can be identified among the nonhuman Primates. Chimpanzees are in fact rapidly becoming the prime exception to the seperatist perspective, as Pan is already known to exhibit more such traits than any extant genus other than Homo itself. Chimpanzees are, for example, known to cooperate in prey pursuit, to practice division of labor in pursuing and capturing prey, to share meat along lines reflecting kinship, consort and other social ties-all within the single context of predation (Teleki, 1973b,c). And, as shown in this report, chimpanzees are also technically skilled Primates which, as Lancaster (1968) has aptly pointed out, makes this living pongid the best candidate for mirroring the ancestor postulated for the origin of the hominid line. Similar advances continue to occur along many avenues of investigation, some of the most outstanding being family structure (van LawickGoodall, 1968, in prep.), community organization (e.g. Itani & Suzuki, 1967) and communication (e.g. Gardner & Gardner, 1969; Rumbaugh, Gill & von Glaserfeld, 1973). Once Homo erectus sites have been passed, in our search backward through time, the archeological record of artefacts becomes skimpy indeed. And, in the few cases where claims for such evidence are made, debates about authenticity continue for decades. But the evidence for technological behavior among living Primates suggests that this is one of many attributes which can be traced far into Primate prehistory. The ability to manipulate objects for specific purposes is probably one of the more ancient and pervasive trends in Primate evolution, having emerged perhaps as early as the Oligocene. Rudimentary kinds of modification and transportation, such as one sees today among baboon and chimpanzee collector-predators, may already have played a part in subsistence activities during the Miocene, although more sophisticated forms of such behavior, as performed by some contemporary human gatherer-hunters, may not have appeared until the Pliocene. Most of the materials utilized by chimpanzees (and by other Primates) in their foodgetting activities are highly maleable and perishable. These materials are in fact often indistinguishable, sometimes even after being modified, from similar materials to be found in the vi&&y. Herein lie important implications for hypotheses which maintain that technological behavior initially arose among Primates when technical skills were applied to ‘hard’ materials that are prone to long-term preservation. Debates about the authenticity of the osteodontokeratic culture may continue for many years, but the likelihood that some extinct Primates once possessed technological capabilities-expressed for perhaps millions of years only in the manipulations, modification and transportation of ‘soft’ materials-is at least circumstantially strengthened by the chimpanzee evidence.
CHIMPANZEE
SUBSISTENCE TECHNOLOGY
593
Final, unequivocal proof for such speculation must, of course, come from the discoveries of paleoanthropologists. The approach which has been taken in this report-wherein acquisition and propagation of technological behavior is viewed as a gradual evolutionary process involving many Primate species-is offered for consideration largely because it appears to be theoretically more satisfying than those which link major evolutionary developments to some single factor (e.g. hominization to predation). Consequently, it is at least concordant with the incremental and cumulative processes of gradual change upon which biological theories of Primate evolution are founded. Hopefully my investigations have also shown that studies of nonhuman Primates can be relevant and instructive to those who seek knowledge about the emergence and evolution of Homo sapiens. I am deeply indebted to the National Geographic Society and the Gombe Stream Research Centre for their financial and logistical support of my field work, and to the Government and National Parks of Tanzania for the opportunity to live in one of their newest conservation areas. I am, in addition, more than grateful to the patient and. tolerant Leakey, whose termite-collecting skills so outstripped mine. References Albrecht, H. & Dunnett, S. C. (1971). Chimpanzeesin Western Africa. Munich: Piper. Azuma, S. (1973). Acquisition and propagation of new food habits in a troop of Japanese monkeys. In (C. R. Carpenter, Ed.), Behavioral Regulators of Behavior in Primates. Lewisburg, Pa.: Bucknell University Press. Progress report of the survey of chimpanzees in their natural habitat,, Azuma, S. & Toyoshima, A. (1961-2). Kabogo Point area, Tanganyika. Primates 3,61-70. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (1972). Field research on baboons, drills and geladas: an historical, geo graphical and bibliographical listing. Primates 13,427-432. Baldwin, L. A. & Teleki, G. (1973). Field research on chimpanzees and gorillas: an historical, geographical and bibliographical listing. Primates 14, 315-330. Beatty, E. H. (1951). A note on the behaviour of the chimpanzee. Journal of Mammalogy 32, 118. Bodenheimer, B. S. (1951). Insects as IIuman Food. The Hague: Dr W. Junk. Eimerl, S. & DeVore, I. (1965). The Primates. New York: Time-Life Books. Fried, M. (1967). The Evolution of Political Society. New York: Random House. Gardner, B. T. & Gardner, R. A. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165,644-672. Goodall, J. ( 1962). Nest building in free-ranging chimpanzees. Annals of New York Academy of Science 102, 455-467. Goodall, J. (1964). Toolusing and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature 201, 1264-1266. Current Anthropolou Hall, K. R. L. (1963). Tool-using performances as indicators of behavioral adaptability. 4,479-494. Hall, K. R. L. (1965). Animals that use tools. Animals 7, 16-2 1. Harding, R. S. 0. (1973). Predation by a troop of olive baboons (Papio anubis). American Journal of Physical Anthropologv 30, 587-592. Itani, J. & Susuki, A. (1967). The social unit of chimpanzees. Primates 8, 355-381. Izawa, K. & Itani, J. (1966). Chimpanzees in Kasakati Basin, Tanganyika: I. Ecological study in the rainy season, 1963-1964. Qoto University African Studies 1, 73-156. Jones, C. & Sabater Pi, J. (1969). Sticks used by chimpanzees in Rio Muni, West Africa. Nature 223, IOO101. Jones, Cl. & Sabater Pi, J. (1971). Comparative ecology of Gorilla gorilla (Savage and Wyman) and Pun troglodytes (Blumenbach) in Rio Mum, West Africa. Bibliographica Primatologica no. 13. Basel: S. Karger. Kohler, W. (1325). The Mentality of Apes. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Kortlandt, A. (1963). Bipedal armed fighting in chimpanzees. XVI International Congress of Zoology Washington, lecture text. Kortlandt, A. (1965). How do chimpanzees use weapons when fighting leopards? Yearbook of American Philosphical Society pp. 327-332.
594
G. TELEKI
Kortlandt, A. (1967). Experimentation with chimpanzees in the wild. In (D. Starck, R. Schneider & H. J. Kuhn, Eds), Neue Ergebnisse der Primatologie. Stuttgart: Fischer. Kortlandt, A. (1968). Handgebrauch bei freilebenden Schimpansen. In (B. Rensch, Ed.), Handgebrauch und Verstiindigung bei Affen und Friihmenschen. Bern: Huber. Kortlandt, A. & Kooij, M. (1963). Protohominid behavior in primates (preliminary communication). Symposia of Zoological Society of London 10,61-88. J. B. On the of tool-using American Anthropologist 56-70. Mann, (1972). Hominid cultural origins. 7, 379-386. E. (1969). Soul of Ape. New Atheneum. Menzel, W. (1972). invention of in a of young Folia Prima17, 87-106. E. W. Further observations the use ladders in group of chimpanzees. Folia 19,450-457. Merfield, G. & H. (1956). Were My London: Longmans. T. (1973). ant-gathering behavior the use tools among chimpanzees of Mahali Mountains. of Human 2, 357-370. K. P. Man the (2nd edit.). University of Press. Pfeiffer, E. (1969). Emergence of New York: and Row. U. (1971). d’outils par chimpanzes de de la Terre et 25, 506-509. T. W. Ecology and behavior of (Pa&o anubis) Gombe National Unpublished Dissertation. of California, Reynolds, V. Reynolds, F. Chimpanzees of Budongo Forest. (I. DeVore, Primate Behavior: Studies of and Apes. York: Holt, and Winston. D. M., T. V. van Glasersfeld, C. (1973). and sentence by a (Pan). Science 731-733. Sahlins, D. (1959). social life monkeys, apes primitive man. (J. N. Ed.), The Wayne State Press. of Man’s for Culture. Savage, S. & J. (1843-4). of the characters and of the niger. Boston of Natural 4, 362-386. G. B. The G/wi In (M. Bicchieri, Ed.), and Gatherers New York: Rinehart and Struhsaker, T. & Hunkeler, (1971). Evidence tool-using by in the Coast. Folia 15, 2 19. Sugiyama, (1968). Social of chimpanzees the Budongo Uganda. Primates 225-258. Sugiyama, (1969). Social ofchimpanzees in the Forest, Uganda. 10,197-225. Suzuki, (1966). On insect-eating habits wild chimpanzees in the woodland of Tanzania. Primates Suzuki, A. An ecological of chimpanzees a savanna Primates 10, Teleki, G. Notes on interactions with carnivores in National Park, Primates 14,407-411. G. (19736). Predatory Behavior Wild Chimpanzees. Pa.: Bucknell Press. Teleki, (19736). The chimpanzee. Scien@ 228,32-42. Teleki, (1973d). On responses to accidental death a chimpanzee Gombe National Tanzania. Folia 20,81-94. Teleki, (in press). subsistence patterns: and gatherer-hunters. of Human van Lawick-Goodall, (1968). The of free-living in the Stream Reserve. Behavior Monographs 16 l-3 van Lawick-Goodall, (1970). Tool-using primates and vertebrates. In S. Lehrman, A. Hinde E. Shaw, Advancesin Study of London: Academic van Lawick-Goodall, van Lawick, & Packer, (1973). Tool-use free-living baboons Gombe National Tanzania. Nature behavior of and early In (S. Washburn, Washburn, S. & DeVore, (1961). S Ed.), Life of Man. Chicago: Zon, J. C. J. van & Orshoven, J. van (1967). Enkele resultaten van de Zesde Nederlandse chimpanseeexpeditie. Vakblad voor Biologien 47, 161-166.