Behavioural Processes, Elsevier
13 (1986) 327-338
DELAYING REINFORCEMENT SUPERSTITIOUS BEHAVIORS1
RITA 6. Department U.S.A. (Accepted
of
327
AN
IN
AUTOSHAPING
TASK
GENERATES
XLEVEN and MESSING, MARK S. Pharmacology, University of Minnesota,
10 July
ADJUNCTIVE
AND
SHELDON 6. SPARBER Minneapolis, Minnesota,
1986)
ABSTRACT Delaying Reinforcement in an Messing, R. B., Kleven, M. S. and Sparber, S. B. Autoshaping Task Generates Adjunctive and Superstitious Behaviors. Behav. Processes, 13: 327-338. Key Words:
adjunctive behavior - autoshaping supersitious behavior
- reinforcemental
delay
-
Rats were autoshaped to touch a lever upon its insertion into an operant chamber on a 45 s random time schedule. Occurrence of a reinforced touch on each of 12 lever insertions per session and nose-pokes at the retracted lever as was exploratory rearing activity. Delays of 2, 4 or 8 s were monitored, interposed between the retraction of the lever, which occurred either after 15 s or after a touch response, and delivery of the food pellet reward, resulted in progressively slower acquisition of the extended lever touch response. However, if rats had already acquired the response under immediate reinforcement the delays subsequently introduced did not cause a decline in conditions, Nose-pokes at the retracted lever occurred during autoshaped touch responding. intervals over the course of both intertrial and reinforcement delay frequency of these autoshaping. The appearance and (adjunctive or superstitious) behaviors depended upon the reinforcement delay and behavioral history. These interval behaviors offer measurements of learning not based on arbitrary criteria; they thus provide information about effects on endpoints, or a more global approach to learning. INTRODUCTION Studies
of reinforcement
demonstrated
learning
a lever press delay (1956)
and
showed
in rats with
response,
rate
of
delay using discrete
that
reinforcement
with the expected
acquisition delay
of
trials operant
(Harker, reward
delays
negative 1956;
does not
of up to
correlation
Perin,
procedures
1943).
affect
have
10 s following
between
length of
Further,
asymptotic
Harker response
f***************************************~~~~~~~*~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~ lThis research was supported in part by a USPHS Mid-Career Development Award-Environmental Toxicoloav 5K07ES00123 to RBM and Grant No. SMF-396-84 from the Minnesota Medical Foindation. We thank J. Bruce Overmier for critical reading of the manuscript. Request for reprints should be sent to Rita B. Messing, Department of Pharmacology, 3-260 Millard Hall, 435 Delaware Street S.E., University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455.
0376-6357/86/$03.50
0 1986 Elsevier
Science
Publishers
B.V. (Biomedical
Division)
328
and
latencies, short
to
a
manifested
as
by
performance
standard it
lever
(Brown
&
(Buzsaki, response
Additionally,
increases
(ITI’s),
& Gamzu,
(Messing
the course
retracted
of
are
lever)
opportunity
adventitiously The
1948).
than
effects
between
development those
under
development
and
convenient
means
global approach response. interval
based
which
of
these
of
analyses
to what
Thus, behaviors
the
behaviors
responding
of
or
operant
a
with
or more
& Sparber,
interim
during
IT1 nose-poke
have
(Falk,
1971;
experiment,
provides
an additional
during
behavior
measurements
which
tend
session
these
of
intervals
may
in endpoints,
on
learning ceiling
Differences
to the required
reward
(Skinner,
to introduce
length.
differences
of
the
class of nose poke behaviors,
in addition
delay
intervals
responses
behavior
In the present
in
lever touching
intertrial
superstitious
responses,
is being learned
than
In essence,
1981),
and food reward provided
of possible
influence
rather
either
Terrace,
and
autoshaping
procedure.
adjunctive
1982).
manageable
of
1974),
a
these
learning
1983; Messing
These or
touching,
on discrete
extends
on
forward
clearly
1977;
of what may be another
maintenance for
not
occurs
progresses.
lever
conditions
shaping
of autoshaping,
lever retraction
reinforced
trial,
& Sparber,
schedule-induced
for measurement
reward
from
decrement
behaviors.
& Sirenelhag, 1971; Wetherington,
delay interposed
operant
experiment
of
& Silberberg,
operant
(operant)
work
during
present delay
an
in a performance
a discrete
which
Schwartz
as learning
characteristics
Stadden
other
1982;
contingent
performing
result
of
Stiers
trials
behaviors,
previous
at the
using
1968;
are
not The
effects
or discrete
has shown that
which
does
latencies.
Jenkins,
respondent
the
in
autoshaped
poking
animals
reward
responding
free running
traditional
(nose
of
investigating
of
compares
press)
shifting
delay
increases
observations
procedure
that
long
development
in
provide
or a more lever touch of
these
was also examined.
METHODS Subjects The subjects were 21 male Long Evans rats obtained from Blue Spruce Farms Rats were gradually food weighing 440-515 g. (Altamont, N.Y.), initially deprived and maintained at 80-85% of initial body weights, in a temperature (22'C) and humidity (40-50%) controlled enviromment on a 12:12 hr light-dark schedule, with lights on at 0700 hr. Testing was done between 0900 and 1200 hr. Apparatus Rats were tested in 4 operant chambers (BRS/LVE Laurel, MD), 31 cm wide x 25 cm deep x 25 cm high, with floors of 0.5 cm diameter stainless steel bars spaced One 25 cm square panel in each chamber contained a food trough and 2 cm apart. a house lamp in the center, near floor and ceiling, respectively, and standard cue lamp configurations above slots for levers on the left and right sides. A retractable lever (BRS/LVE, Laurel, MD) was mounted in the left hand slot, while Chambers also contained speakers through which the right hand slot was covered. Each box was equipped with a metal strip a white masking noise was introduced.
329
(7.5 cm wide, 12 cm above the floor) on the two walls opposite the lever and opposite the door. Contacts with the strip, representing rearing activity along the walls, were monitored with a drinkometer circuit. Contacts with the lever either in an extended (touches) or retracted position (nose-poke responses) were likewise monitored with a drinkometer circuit. The drinkometer circuits were standardized against a 2 MU resistance to assure equal sensitivity between chambers and from day to day. Chambers were also cleaned with 70% (v/v) ethanol, and allowed to dry between subjects to prevent changes in sensitivity due to residual oils or salts. The experimental chambers were enclosed in custom made sound attenuating cubicles equipped with closed circuit video cameras, used to observe rats (Sparber, 1979). Control of the experimental chambers and collection of data were accomplished with microcomputers and cassette recorders (Tandy Corporation, Fort Worth, Texas) connected to the operant chambers by custom built interfaces. Procedure Three days before the first autoshaping session, rats were allowed to eat ten 45 mg food pellets (Bioserv, Frenchtown, NJ), placed in a food trough in wire mesh cages which were similar to the ones in which rats were housed. All This procedure was used instead of rats consumed the pellets within 25 min. magazine training, and insured that rats were familiar with the pellet reinforcers in the initial autoshaping session. They were not exposed to the experimental chambers prior to acquisition training. Rats were randomly assigned to one of four groups for autoshaping, the 8 s delay of reinforcement group having 6 subjects instead of 5. During the experimental sessions, a white cue light was illuminated above the lever. Extension of the lever into the chamber served as an exteroceptive stimulus and its retraction was followed, after various delays, with food pellet delivery. Lever insertions occurred according to a random time schedule, with an average 45 s (range 22-68 s) interval between food delivery and the next extension of The lever remained extended for 15 s, or until a lever touch was the lever. made, after which it was immediately retracted. The four groups were trained under different conditons of delay of food One group received reinforcement pellet delivery following lever retraction. simultaneously with lever retraction (0 s delay). Other groups were reinforced lever retractions. Rats were exposed to 12 lever 2, 4 or 8 s after presentations (trials) per day, five days per week. When all rats in a group reached the criterion of at least 11 extended lever touch responses in a 12 trial session in three of four consecutive sessions, the group was shifted to the next highest reinforcement delay interval, for the next three sessions. The delay intervals were then increased once every three sessions, regardless of animals' levels of performance (see Table 1).
TABLE
1
Delay of Reinforcement
Designated (Original OS
2s 4s 8s
by Session
SESSION
Group Delay)
l-7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16-18
0 2 4 8
2 2 4 8
2 2 4 8
2 2 4 8
4 2 4 8
4 4 4 8
4 4 4 8
8 4 4 8
8 8 4 8
8 8 8 8
Touches of the extended lever, latency/l2 trials for each session (180s maximum), interval nose-pokes
to touch the extended lever of the retracted lever, and
330
unconditioned rearing (strip touch activity) were obtained for each twelve trial When a lever touch was not made before the lever was retracted, 15 s session. In addition, in 12 of was used for calculation of total extended touch latency. by trial printout of nose-pokes during each the 18 sessions, a trial permitting separate analyses of reinforcement delay and IT1 was obtained, These data were not retrievable for Sessions 2, responding during these times. 3, 6, 15, 17 and 18, because of malfunction of the data recording equipment (see Figures 3 and 4). Data Analyses Data for extended lever touches, touch latencies, interim nose pokes at the lever, and strip touches were analyzed in blocks of 12 trials (i.e. across experimental sessions) by two way (Group x Session) repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) (Winer, 1971). Strip touch and nose poke analyses were performed on square root transformed data to eliminate heterogeneity of variance. Since some groups were shifted to longer reinforcement delays at various times after the seventh session, separate ANOVAs were performed on data obtained in Sessions l-7 and Sessions 8-18 (see Table 1). A criterion of p < 0.05 was used for rejection of the null hypothesis. RESULTS
Extended
Lever Touching
As expected, was
inversely
delay
the
rate
related
of reward
to the delay
(0 s group)
delay of reinforcement groups
trained
responses Session
during
(E[18,
consistently the
first =
rats
The group means
an
8 s delay,
(evidence will
autoshape
Data are similar
scores,
The
for this group was
lo-12 touch
performed
An ANOVA
for mean to quanta1
by
responses
11-12 touches 16-18 ranged
this
(F_[17, 851
group
a significant
rats
8
in
the
during
Session
the
15, the
6.5; however,
s
1).
The
scores
effect x
in
of
Session
group
never
18 sessions
of
mean
of
number
4 of the 6 rats
in each of the last 3 sessions, one
no
lever touch
main
Group
between
and
of these
sessions.
7.8 and 8.5 correct
responses
for the group of rats trained with
did
= 2.03,
criterion
with
to a 2 s
1, Table
significant
in at least
on all sessions,
that
an 8 s delay,
shorter
example,
them
ANOVA on extended
as a group,
responses
revealed
with
Pc.01).
a
(Figure
attained
showed and
For
of learning)
with the,three
sessions
P<.OOl),
2.60,
for Sessions
out of 12 trials.
delays
response
The rats trained
session
shown).
lever touch
of these
seven
criterion
(not
in the 8 s group made all
=
12-trial
lever touch
for switching
The repeated measures
19.04,
1021
attained
experiment
extended
the 1021
extended
the criterion
reinforcement
(if at all).
(E[6,
interaction
longer
of the
of reinforcement.
reached
by the seventh
with
later sessions
of acquisition
indeed
PC.03).
but the process
show
an effect
It thus takes
many
of Session
appears more
that
rats
trials
than
delays. latency/l2
trials
data for extended
to touch
the extended
lever responding.
lever
(Figure
2)
If only the first
331
8
8
X-X
0 set
c+-o
2 set
8--8
4 set
t-a
8 set I
I
I
I
1
2
3
4
I
I
I
5
6
7
SESSION Acquisition of autoshaped lever touch responding. Each session Fig. 1. represents 12 lever presentations (trials) made on an 45 s random time schedule. Groups were trained with different delays between lever retraction and food pellet reward.
7 sessions delay
are considered,
Group
Session
(E[6,
interaction response to
an
(i.e.
there
collapsed
1021
(E[18,
=
19.6,
minimum;
group,
did show a significant
(![17,
851 = 3.08, Pc.001).
first
15 sessions,
evidence Sessions evident
for
change,
Pc.001).
decrease
as
a
a
173
for reinforcement 3.30,
P<.O5),
Session
and
x
Group
As was the case for the quanta1
touch
ANOVA
for the 8 s group did not fall
for
in latency
all
18 sessions
to respond
for
across
the
8 s
18 sessions
data of this group for the
Sessions
1-15
However,
response
the conclusion
=
significant
like the quanta1
for
group.
(E[3,
latencies
an
Again,
latencies
16-18, confirming
(see Figure
that response
2) show
latencies acquisition
fell
little during
was becoming
during the last 3 sessions.
No decreases rats were shifted no
the
and
lever touch however,
main effects
Sessions)
P<.OOl),
1021 = 2.89,
data, the extended
asymptotic
are significant
across
marked
in the
number
of extended
to longer delays
increases
in response
lever
after attaining latencies
responses criterion
were observed
were
observed
(not shown).
for the
trained
when Also, rats
332
x 3
180
0 set
x--x O--O
2 set
H
4 set
O---O
8 set
‘I
I
:
I
I
I
I
3
1
I
5
II,
I
7
9
I,,
I
I
I
11
13
I
I
15
17
SESSION
Total latency/l2 trials to touch the extended lever as a function of Fig. 2. experimental session and delay of reward during the acquisition phase. Each lever presentation lasted for 15 s, or until a correct lever touch response was made. The dashed vertical after Session 7 indicates that designated groups were shifted to longer reinforcement delays at various times after this Session (see Table 1).
when
they
although they
were naive
have been
to respond
switched rats
to
learn
successfully
at asymptotic,
longer
reinforcement
an 8 s delay autoshaped
short
of with
latencies
delays
reinforcement a shorter
(Figure task
delay
as the delay
2).
very
they will
is increased
Thus,
slowly,
if
continue
from 0 to 8
5. It
thus
appears
produce
varying
trained
with
that
response
values
2 or 4 s delays
than they do to rats trained a significant when
the
responses
change
number
of
sessions
spread between
reinforcement
between
The acquisition
(i.e.
to those trained
required
as an effect
for
is considered
groups
to
8 s,
with no delay is measured
of Session). achieve
as a criterion,
and an 8 session
0 and
rates for subjects
with an 8 s delay, when acquisition
0 and 2 s delay groups,
(Table 1).
of
rates.
appear more similar
in performance
for 3 of 4 sessions
delay groups
of delay
acquisition
there
spread
11-12
as
However, correct
is a 4 session
between
0 and 4 s
333
Interval
Nose-Poking
Increasing behavioral
the
reinforcement
consequences.
1, 4, 5 and 7, (Figure 51]=
4.16,
Pc.02).
ANOVA's
Pc.01). responding (E[3, not
511's
increase
reinforcement
for
response
of nose poking
3, left) shows a significant
a significant
for simple
changed
delay
An analysis
with
main
session
= 7.91 and 4.57, nose-poke
of session
3, right).
of Session
with
during
delays with
(E[3,
showed of
that
2 or 4 s
a 0 s delay did as the delay
ITI’s,
An analysis
other
(r[9, 511 = 3.30,
for each group
The rats trained
significantly
(Figure
interaction
in rats trained
P's c.01).
responding
was increased
x Group
effects
has
for Sessions
ITI’s
main effect
Session
only
acquisition
during
of nose-poking
of
data
Noncontingent lever responding during ITI’s as a function of Fig. 3. experimental session, and delay of reward during the acquisition phase. The dashed vertical after Session 7 indicates that designated groups were shifted to longer reinforcement delays at various times after this Session (see Table 1).
available Group
for Sessions
(5[3,
173
nose-poking
in
Session
despite
changed
7,
between
8-16 showed
= 4.27, ITI’s
P<.O2),
remained the
Sessions
fact
no effect and
no
essentially that
7 and 16.
of Session,
interaction. unchanged
reinforcement
a significant Thus, from
delays
levels were
effect of
it appears attained
that by
systematically
334
F
a
w’ 140 n 5 g 120
x--x
0 set
O--O
2 set
O-0
4 set
O--a 8 set
e g IL 100 Z ii 80
Z Z r
60
3; E B ’
40
i Z
I
I
I
1
I
I
I
I
5
I
t
I
I
10
I
1
15
SESSION Noncontingent lever responding during reinforcement delay intervals Fig. 4. as a function of experimental session and delay of reward during the acquisition phase. The dashed vertical after Session 7 indicates that designated groups this Session (see Table 1). shifted to longer reinforcement delays after were
An
analysis
variance
of
performed
reinforcement
delay
intervals
Sessions
since
no measurements
8-16
sessions. Session 2.97,
There
PC.01).
the extended appeared
were
main
(E[7, 1191 = 6.79,
lever touch
delay.
However,
response
without
a delay
lever during this portion
exist
for
only for
the
of Group
and a significant
groups
response,
considerably
of the experiment.
during
the
available for
(E[3, 173 = 10.88,
the
P<.OOl),
interaction
from
first
7
and of
(E[21, 1191 =
performance
scores
rates during the reinforcement
of reinforcement, during
poking data
0 s group
reached maximum
response
responding showed
that
nose
includes
effects
a 2-4 s delay
as with
4)
P<.OOl),
For all three
to peak with
(Figure
for
delay
and to fall with a 4-8 s
ITI's, the group less activity
that
toward
acquired
the
the retracted
335
Rats were
switched
nose-poking
were
experiment,
it appears
Sessions
l-7
behavior
to higher
attained.
had
before
effect
occured
on
either
asymptotic
within
that the reinforcement
a marked
which
delays
Nevertheless,
delay
emergence
during
the
levels of interim
limits
schedule
of
and
intertrial
of
the
subsequent
or
present
experienced
during
nose-poking
reinforcement
delay
intervals. Behaviors differently relatively delays
real,
0,
after
4).
An
tended
numbers
Fig.
contrast
delay
3 for the
Session
4.
during
of
responding
missing
data
extended shows
for
these
reinforcement other the
reinforcement for the
Session
animals
delays
groups),
but
reinforcement
Rearing
delays
may
influenced remained
reinforcement
acquired be more
the behavior apparent
reinforcement
delays
than
changed
for 0, 2 and 4 s groups,
time
lever
4 shows
Fig.
and
nose-poking
this
there
did
group
levels
of Figs.
4.
in
(Evaluation
because
of nose
of the
increase
poking
and basal
in
is always
during
levels
at low levels
responding
rate of nose-poking
lever
an increase
a marked
8 s group
remained between
increase
3 and 4 for the 0 s group
amounts
for the ITI’s
difference
showed
not
1 with
in extended
was
to
behaviors
of Fig.
problematical
considerable
during
the
increase
that
is more
as opposed
of these
in this group in Session
examination
poking
intertrial
Comparison
touching.
intertrial
delays
Also,
during
of development
there was a marked
exhibited
is that
responding
in which
6,
Another
in reinforcement
for
throughout
in
ITI's
and
apparently
much
delays.
Activity
No effects activity.
of the
present
procedure
The number of strip touches
between
groups.
Initial
the 2 s group to 4.5/min varied
during
been
responding
rates during
effect
increased
4 s group
(e.g. above nose
experiment.
higher
were
that
However,
lever touching.)
that
shows
5, even though
nose-poking interim
latter
is the
of extended
2 s group,
until after Session by
response
responses
between
intervals,
to development
touching
of
have
IT1
in all groups which
this
as delays
may
however.
to rise slightly.
interesting
relative
intervals
7, while
of delay,
However,
absolute
reinforcement
nor
Session
less than did rates,
actually
different experience,
as a function
2 and
since
these
subsequent
constant
changed
(Groups
much
during by
between
2.6/min
mean
were
varied
observed neither
rates of rearing
for the 4 s group.
on
exploratory
rearing
as a function .of sessions, ranged
between
3.2/min
At the end of the experiment,
for the 2 s group to 4.7/min
for
means
for the 0 s group.
DISCUSSION The
expected
acquisition
relationship
was found.
between
reinforcement
The data are in agreement
delay
and
speed
of
with those of Perin (1943) who
336
found s,
relatively
with
10
consistent with
s with
discrete
on
remained
high
longer
delay
pellet
variables.
three
studies
resemble
have
investigated
which
governing
depend
instrumental The
contingency
behavior
(Skinner,
1948).
the
will
lengthened,
since
readily
Staddon
autoshaping) behavior
importance of
and
phase
the
term
or
as a class
periods
of low
interval
relationships behaviors,
see
and behavior,
and
reinforcement during
during
or
delay
its
during
of
training,
reinforcement
directed
density,
then
by
"supertitious" for
behavior
variability,
cf.
this interval both
rats trained
with
if the delay
a 0
is later
with a 2 or 4 set of IT1 nose-poking
only.
If,
following
(and
by
extension
schedules,
at appropriate
and
reinforcement
as suggested
during
determinant
conditioning
delay IT1
or CS-US interval,
Thus,
behaviors
the major
the
from the other terminal
emphasizes
nose-poke
the
reinforced,
is used, behavior
classical
reinforcement
more
instrumental
using a classical
of
this behavior and
of reinforcement
as behavior
which
motoric
CS-US
"superstitious"
and later.
In contrast,
is defined
is defined
changes
or
response,"
adventitiously
terminology
(1971),
and
Harker(1956)
temporal
behaviors
behaviors
touch,
Simmelhag
on the
antecedent
increase
classical
conditioned
of a "terminal
as
these
or
a
throughout.
precise
of the reinforcement
exhibit
effect
retraction
schedule
but to a lesser extent than rats that were trained
to be the delay &
the
it delineates
acquisition
with
of the two).
prefer
lever
as a function
of reinforcement.
appears
(We
extended
response
or
lever
of the apparatus
maintenance
to be part
by starting
(and of interval
is nominally
Nose-poke
(1971),
1966.) Whichever,
varies
set delay
during
the
response
demonstrates and
also were
reinforcement
the
no
as delays
(1943)
other
of
topography
interval.
interval, -
Herrnstein,
during
delay
Simmelhag
this
clearly
also
be considered
component
delay
(or a mixture
acquisition
reinforcement
and
of the
of
of
had
responding
generally
Perin
be that
of a specified
not
delay
delay
ITI
motivational
unchanged
of
results
or not the response
for
may
those
therefore
on details
experiment
schedule
delay
It may
acquisition
not on whether
during
remained
resemble
1961).
do
also
4 s delays
of introducing
behaviors
are
conditioning
of an adverse
on
of up to 5
with
to a longer
emission,
effect
the consequences
operant
not differ
produced
the result
an
behaviors
2 or
response
of
results
(Kimble,
Staddon
than
nose-poke
since rearing
procedures
delay
rather
likely
results
experiment,
with
performance
between
delays
The
using
did
a short
present
poorer
is most
found,
from
trained
present
all
below)
the
who
rats
with
acquisition.
latencies
In the rats
association(s)
maintain
behaviors,
than
in
Furthermore,
or
The
switching
of reward,
of
(1956)
asymptotic
latency.
of an operant
slower
Harker
that
Thus,
delivery,
induce
of
rates
increased.
formation
on learning
markedly
and that
response at
further
those
trials,
reinforcement, effect
small effects producing
stimuli
nose-poking
and adjunctive (goal objects) directed
at the
337
retracted
lever
during
ITI's
may
be
viewed
as
schedule-induced
adjunctive
behavior. The
results
response (i.e.
with
rates
of
rates
interval
history
of
of the
of animals
determinant
administration,
which
clinical
to
effectiveness
summary,
In
results
experiments
perform
at
of drugs
response
without
asymptotic
the course
is strongly reaching
is 2-4 s.
the conditions its
several
conditioned
convenient (e.g.
and
drugs,
rapid
The and
apparent
task
is
with
switched
brain
which
occurred)
amendable
lesions,
use
learning
delay
to
of
the
acquired
will
long
(8
continue s)
and history
of
reinforcement (i.e.
determinant
difficulty
be measured, of
of
develop
of an organism
in
to
of the delay of
training
influence
etc.) on learning
delays
behaviors
is the
and the appearance
variations may
repeated potential
acquisition
may be an important
behaviors
in
may not
deficits.
delays,
to
repeated
Such
have already
the
be a
or memory
extinction,
superstitious
when
might
in predicting
a short
or
behavioral
tasks
subjects.
rats which
by the
(e.g. drug or toxicant
on
naive
interposed
studies
that
learning
shows that the history
unconditioned for
to expect
with cognitive
maximum
acquisition
method
toxicants,
with
influenced
the rate of autoshaped
being
adjunctive
the study
under which
performance.
conditions.
utility
related to the magnitude
an
Finally,
or
after
of autoshaping
reinforcement,
of
a delay
levels
in different
have experienced with
in patients
Additionally,
engaging
experiments
differential
of reinforcement,
these behaviors
delay
experiments have
touch
optimally,
environmental
or other
manipulations
it was found that although
reinforcement. during
such
lever
response
to be greatly
reasonable
to learn and/or of
slower with longer delays contingent
appear
Therefore,
of
may
even while
of other manipulations
lesions).
extended
this
external
to autoshaping
effects
the
perform
latency)
is thus
It
acquired
will
similar
effects
have learned
comparable
acquisition
study
have
responding
animal.
brain
to
under
subjected
of performance
acquisition
which
conditions
in response
behaviors
past
be
rats
delay
no decrement
is,
subjects
that
interval
That
history
show
under different
making
diverse
and it a
factors
and memory.
REFERENCES Brown, P. L. and Jenkins, H. M., 1968. Autoshaping of the pigeon's key peck. J. Exp. Anal. Behav., ll:l-8. Buzsaki, G., 1982. The "where is it?" reflex: Autoshaping the orienting J. Exp. Anal. Behav., 37:461-484. response. Falk, J. L., 1971. The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiol. & Behav., 6:577-588. Delay of reward and performance of an instrumental Harker, G. S., 1956. response. J. Exp. Psychol. 51:303-310. Herrnstein, R. J.,1966. Superstition: A corollary of the principles of operant conditioning. In: W. K. Honig (Editor), Operant Behavior, Areas of Research and Application. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp. 33-51. Kimble, G A., 1961. Hilgard and Marquis' Conditioning and Learning. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York. Messing, R. B. and Sparber, S. B., 1983. Des-gly-vasopressin improves
338
acquisition and slows extinction of autoshaped behavior. Eur. J. Pharmacol., 89:43-51. Messing, R. B. and Sparber, S. B., 1985. Greater task difficulty amplifies the facilitatory effect of des-glycinamide arginine vasopressin upon learning. Behav. Neurosci. appetitively motivated 99:1114-1119. Perin, C. T., 1943. A quantitative investigation of the delay-of-reinforcement J. Exp. Psychol. 32:37-51. gradient. Schwartz, B. and Gamzu, E., 1977. Pavlovian control of operant behavior. In: W. K. Honig and J. E. R. Staddon (Editors), Handbook of Operant Conditioning. Prentice-Hall, England Cliffs, NJ, pp. 53-97. Superstition in the pigeon. J. Exp. Psychol., 38:168Skinner, B. F., 1948. 172. Use of learned behavior in testing for neurotoxicity. Sparber, S. B., 1979. In: R. M. Gryder and V. H. Frankos (Editors), Effects of Foods and Drugs on the Development and Function of the Nervous System: Methods for Predicting Toxicity, Proceedings of the Fifth FDA Science Symposium. Office of Health Affairs, FDA, Washington, D.C., pp. 49-61. Staddon, J. E. R. and Simmelhag, V. L., 1971. The "superstition" experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychol. Rev. 78:3-43. Stiers, M. and Silberberg, A., 1974. Lever-contact responses in rats: Automaintenance with and without a negative response - reinforcer dependency. J. Exp.Anal. Behav., 22:497-506. Terrace, H. S., 1981. Introduction: Autoshaping and two-factor learning theory. In: C. M. Locurto, H. S. Terrace and J. Gibbon (Editors), Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory. Academic Press, New York, pp. 1-18. Wetherinqton, C. L., 1982. Is adjunctive behavior a third class of behavior?. Neurosci. Biobehav..Rev., 6:329-350. Winer, B. J., 1971. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, 2nd edition. McGraw Hill, New York.