Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
www.jahonline.org Original article
Evaluation of the Start Strong Initiative: Preventing Teen Dating Violence and Promoting Healthy Relationships Among Middle School Students Shari Miller, Ph.D. *, Jason Williams, Ph.D., Stacey Cutbush, M.A., Deborah Gibbs, M.P.H., Monique Clinton-Sherrod, Ph.D., and Sarah Jones, M.P.H. RTI International, Research Triangle Park, North Carolina
Article history: Received March 6, 2014; Accepted November 6, 2014 Keywords: Teen dating violence; Dating abuse; Early adolescence; Prevention; Dating
A B S T R A C T
Purpose: This study reports on an independent evaluation of Start Strong: Building Healthy Teen Relationships, a multicomponent initiative targeting 11- to 14-year-olds. “Start Strong” was designed to focus on the developmental needs of middle school students and to enhance skills and attitudes consistent with promotion of healthy relationships and reduction of teen dating violence (TDV). Methods: The quasi-experimental evaluation design included data collection from four Start Strong schools and four comparison schools. Student surveys were collected at four waves of data at the beginning and the end of grades 7 and 8. Multilevel models used repeated observations nested within students who were, in turn, nested within schools to determine whether participation in Start Strong enhanced healthy skills and relationships and decreased TDV-related attitudes and behaviors. Results: Short-term effects from waves 1 to 2 were statistically significant for increased parentechild communication and boy/girlfriend relationship satisfaction and support and decreased gender stereotypes and attitudes supporting TDV. Findings for acceptance of TDV and gender stereotypes persisted longitudinally. Conclusions: Results are promising and illustrate that a multicomponent, community-based initiative reduced risk factors predictive of TDV. Start Strong is innovative in its focus on early adolescence, which is a critical period in the transition to dating. The results inform future intervention efforts and underscore the need for further study of middle school students. Ó 2015 Society for Adolescent Health and Medicine. All rights reserved.
Teen dating violence (TDV) is gaining recognition as a serious public health problem. Nationally, about 1 in 11 high school students (9.4%) report having been physically hit, slapped, or physically hurt on purpose by a boyfriend or girlfriend [1]. TDV in adolescence has negative health consequences, such as alcohol and substance
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Disclaimer: Publication of this article was supported by the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. The opinions or views expressed in this article are those of the author and do not necessarily represent the official position of the funders. * Address correspondence to: Shari Miller, Ph.D., RTI International, 3040 Cornwallis Road, P.O. Box 12194, Research Triangle Park, NC 27709-2194. E-mail address:
[email protected] (S. Miller). 1054-139X/Ó 2015 Society for Adolescent Health and Medicine. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.11.003
use, depression, disordered eating, pregnancy, and suicidal ideation, as well as involvement in violent relationships as a young adult [2,3]. Several programs have documented outcomes in responding to TDV. The Safe Dates and Fourth R programs are two schoolbased curricula that demonstrate effectiveness in reducing TDV [4,5]. Safe Dates evidenced reduced psychological, moderate physical and sexual TDV perpetration, and moderate physical TDV victimization at all four follow-up periods [4]. Findings for the Fourth R at the 2.5-year follow-up period revealed that fewer students receiving the intervention reported physical TDV, as compared to the control group. This effect was due to decreased physical TDV by male students receiving the Fourth R; effects were not significant for female students [5]. Another
S. Miller et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
school-based programd“Shifting Boundaries”dincluded a curriculum and a building-based intervention consisting of increased faculty security in school hot spots and temporary school-based restraining orders [6]. Students in the building-only condition and the combined conditions (both curriculum and building intervention) reported reduced sexual violence victimization by peers or dating partners. Students in the building-only condition reported decreased sexual violence perpetration by peers. Families for Safe Dates consists of booklets with TDV-related activities for parents and teens between the ages of 13 and 15 years. The curriculum led to reductions in physical TDV perpetration and victimization [7]. Another program, “Coaching Boys into Men,” used high school athletic coaches to reduce TDV among their athletes. At 1-year follow-up, athletes receiving the intervention reported less TDV perpetration [8]. This small but growing body of literature reveals a number of gaps. First, with one exception (Shifting Boundaries), interventions were developed and tested with high school students. Yet, the limited empirical base on middle school students reveals TDV rates that are comparable to rates of high school students [9]. Young adolescence is a critical period when early dating affiliations first unfold [10]. Prevention in middle school can disrupt TDV before young teens begin to date or before when behaviors become more severe [11]. Second, interventions to prevent TDV predominate the literature, and little is known about efforts to promote healthy relationships. Third, less is known about the effectiveness of TDV preventive interventions in low socioeconomic urban communities [12]. A final gap is the dearth of multicomponent initiatives, which have shown positive effects for other risk behaviors (e.g., Seattle Social Development Project; Hawkins et al. [13]). Such initiatives have the advantage of targeting multiple contexts and strategies that may be necessary to affect change. The present study addresses several of these gaps. It reports on an independent evaluation of Start Strong: Building Healthy Teen Relationships initiative (Start Strong; http://startstrong. futureswithoutviolence.org/), a multicomponent initiative targeting 11- to 14-year-olds. Start Strong was designed to focus on the developmental needs of middle school students and to enhance skills and attitudes consistent with promotion of healthy relationships and reduction of TDV. Program elements included (1) implementing school-based TDV prevention curricula; (2) engaging key influencers (parents/caregivers, teachers, other mentors) to help young teens understand healthy relationships; (3) using social marketing strategies; and (4) working on policy and environmental change. One illustration of key influencers was engaging high school students as “ambassadors” who talked about the program’s merits with their younger peers. For social marketing, the sites relied on Facebook to communicate messages about healthy relationships and TDV. Policy changes included efforts to add TDV to existing sexual harassment or bullying policies. Start Strong included several components to address the multiple levels of risk impacting TDV. This approach is consistent with public health initiatives targeting other health-comprising behaviors, such as youth violence (Multisite Violence Prevention Project [14,15]). Sites were permitted to choose either the Safe Dates [16] or the Fourth R [5] curricula [17]. These two curricula were the only available evidence-based programs targeting TDV when Start Strong began. Elements other than the curricula varied, such as parent engagement, social marketing strategies, and policy change efforts. Such elements were defined by sites in accordance with Start Strong’s community-driven focus.
S15
This study focuses on those schools implementing the Safe Dates curriculum with seventh graders to investigate the effects of the initiative. The objectives of this study were to evaluate if Start Strong enhanced healthy skills and relationships (communication and response to anger skills, parentechild communication, quality of boy/girlfriend relationship) and decreased TDV-related attitudes and behaviors (acceptance of TDV, negative gender stereotypes, perceived negative consequences of TDV, TDV victimization, and perpetration). Methods Study design The quasi-experimental evaluation design matched comparison schools to intervention schools on the following criteria: metropolitan area characteristics, school size, percent students on free/reduced lunch, and race/ethnicity. Within the pool of feasible comparison sites, we matched on these variables as closely as possible. Given variability in program components, site-selection criteria were defined to maximize consistency for the evaluation. Criteria included implementation of the same evidence-based curriculum to seventh graders, a minimum of 100 students per grade (for statistical power), and feasibility of participation in the evaluation. Three sites that implemented Safe Dates met these criteria, collectively representing midsized and large urban areas, racial/ethnic diversity, and regional diversity (one site with two schools; two sites with one school). Across the eight schools (four Start Strong and four comparison) the proportion of students on free/reduced school lunch ranged from 43% to 95%. Student data were collected from middle school students at four time points (waves 1e4): fall and spring of grade 7 and fall and spring of grade 8. Although all the intervention schools implemented Safe Dates, the evaluation assesses the comprehensive initiative because policy, parent engagement, and social marketing components were implemented as well. Procedures Students were given a letter from research staff for their parent(s), which explained the study and included a consent form to be returned by the student to school. Students received a $5 gift card incentive for returning the forms regardless of permission status. Teachers who collected 80% of students’ signed parent permission forms, regardless of permission status, received a $40 gift card incentive. Eligibility criteria included being able to complete the questionnaire in English or Spanish, and not being in a self-contained classroom. Surveys were administered in groups during regular school hours using a paper-and-pencil questionnaire. At the beginning of data collection, project staff read an assent script and obtained passive student assent. Procedures were approved by RTI International’s Institutional Review Board. Participants A total of 2,626 students were eligible to participate. The overall participation rate was 58% (48%e71% across schools). The sample included 1,517 students (50% male; 23% white; 28% African-American; 32% Latino; and 17% other/multiple races/ ethnicities). Approximately 96% of the sample was aged 12 or 13 years at the baseline assessment. As with all longitudinal studies, there was some attrition over time. Across the time points,
S16
S. Miller et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
overall attrition was approximately 15% (waves 1 to 2, 4.7%; waves 2 to 3, 7.9%; waves 3 to 4, 3.3%). Most attrition was due to students withdrawing from the school (rather than not taking the survey) and was somewhat less common for Start Strong students (odds ratio [OR], .70; 95% confidence interval [CI], .53e.93)1. Attrition did not vary by gender (OR, .88; 95% CI, .67e1.16). Compared to white students, attrition was more likely in African-Americans (OR, 2.01; 95% CI, 1.33e3.04) and those with other multiple or unreported race/ethnicity (OR, 2.20; 95% CI, 1.40e3.46). Measures Students completed measures on TDV behaviors and other constructs shown to be related to these behaviors. Unless noted otherwise, measures were from the original Safe Dates evaluation [4]. TDV perpetration and victimization. We assessed six TDV outcomes (parallel perpetration and victimization scales for three types of TDV: psychological, physical, and electronic). Items were rated on a four-point scale: never, one to three times, four to nine times, and 10 or more times. Reliabilities were evaluated with Cronbach alpha from baseline data. The psychological and physical TDV scales began with “How many times in the last 6 months have you done these things to a boyfriend or girlfriend?” (perpetration) or “How many times in the last 6 months has a boyfriend or girlfriend done these things to you?” (victimization). Psychological TDV scales included five items each (e.g., “insulted them in front of others” [perpetration]; “insulted you in front of others” [victimization]). Cronbach alphas for psychological TDV scales were a ¼ .80 (perpetration) and a ¼ .86 (victimization). Physical TDV scales were also assessed with five items each (e.g., “pushed, grabbed, shoved, or kicked them” [perpetration]; “pushed, grabbed, shoved, or kicked you” [victimization]). Cronbach alphas for physical TDV scales were a ¼ .90 (perpetration) and a ¼ .89 (victimization). The electronic TDV items were adapted from existing surveys (Tween Dating Relationships Survey [18]; Youth Internet Safety Survey [19]). Eight items asked “How many times in the last 6 months have you done these things to a boyfriend or girlfriend using a cell phone, e-mail, IM, text messaging, Web chat, a blog, or a networking site like MySpace or Facebook?” (perpetration) and “How many times in the last 6 months has a boyfriend or girlfriend done these things to you using a cell phone, e-mail, IM, text messaging, Web chat, a blog, or a networking site like MySpace or Facebook?” (victimization). Electronic TDV scales were assessed with eight items each (e.g., “showed private or embarrassing pictures/video of them to others” (perpetration); “showed private or embarrassing pictures/video of you to others” (victimization)). Cronbach alphas for electronic TDV scales were a ¼ .86 (perpetration) and a ¼ .85 (victimization). Gender stereotypes. Students rated their agreement on 11 statements related to traditional gender norms (e.g., “it is alright for a girl to ask a boy out on a date”; four-point scale ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree”; a ¼ .65).
1 Students enrolled in the study were not followed if they changed to nonparticipating schools.
Acceptance of dating violence. Students rated their acceptance of dating violence for 10 items (e.g., “a girl who makes her boyfriend jealous on purpose deserves to be hit”; four-point scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree; a ¼ .80). Perceived negative consequences. Students rated their agreement with eight statements about what might happen if they perpetrated violence toward a boy/girlfriend (e.g., “if I hit a boyfriend/ girlfriend, she/he would break up with me”; four-point scale ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree; a ¼ .67). Responses to anger. This 12-item scale asked students “In the last 6 months, when you were mad at someone, how often did you do or feel the following things” (e.g., “I screamed at the person I was mad at”; four-point scale ranging from “very often” to “never”; a ¼ .69). Communication skills. Students were asked how often they used seven positive communication skills in the last 6 months (e.g., “ask the person how he or she felt”; four-point scale ranging from very often to never; a ¼ .85). Parentechild communication about relationships. This 14-item measure was adapted from the ParenteAdolescent Communication Scale that assessed the frequency of communication about sex [20]. Students rated how often in the past 6 months they talked with their parents about relationships (e.g., “what it’s like when you first start liking a boy or girl in a different way than a regular friend”; four-point scale ranging from very often to never; a ¼.90). Boy/girlfriend relationship quality. Relationship quality was measured using four scales from the Network Relationships Inventory [21]. Students rated the quality of their relationship on four 3-item subscales (five-point response options ranging from “none” to “most”): satisfaction (“satisfied with your relationship with this person”; a ¼ .91), support (e.g., “turned to this person for support with personal problems”; a ¼ .86), dominance (e.g., “get you to do things his/her way”; a ¼ .70), and criticism (e.g., “point out your faults or put you down”; a ¼ .81).
Analysis Two types of multilevel models were used to evaluate Start Strong effects. Both models used repeated observations nested within students who were in turn nested within schools. First, longitudinal growth models (LGMs) estimated group differences in linear changes over time. The first set of LGM examined shortterm effects from waves 1 to 2. Long-term effects were estimated with an LGM that included data from waves 1 to 4. Linear slopes were included in all models. Quadratic slopes were evaluated for the wave 1 to 4 models. However, this added slope component yielded no gain in inference (i.e., conclusions about group differences did not change), and in most cases, this added complexity resulted in computational problems because of overextraction of the data. All models controlled for gender and race/ethnicity at the individual level. Exploratory models found no evidence of differential attrition or response to outcomes. Thus, missing data were assumed to be missing at random [22] and were accommodated by the maximum likelihood estimator in the models [23].
S. Miller et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
The growth models yielded four parameters relevant to evaluation of main effects. Data were coded such that model estimates were directly interpretable. The intercept indicated the baseline status of the comparison group; the intervention group estimate captured wave 1 differences between the Start Strong and the comparison groups. The time parameter estimated the change from wave 1 to follow-up (either wave 2 or wave 4) in the comparison group. The group by time interaction estimated the difference in change over time between the intervention and comparison groups. This last parameter, the interaction of group and time, was the primary estimate of intervention effects. The second multilevel model relaxed the LGM assumption of linear change and instead examined group differences across the aggregated follow-up assessments, controlling for baseline. All models were estimated with SAS PROC GLIMMIX (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) with a normal response distribution for all variables. For LGM, four random effects or variance components were includeddrandom intercepts and time effects at the individual level (i.e., each student could have different slopes and baseline values), and random intercepts and time effects at the school leveldto reflect the nonindependence of observations nested within schools. Only school-level random intercepts were included in the aggregate follow-up model.
Results Short-term results Table 1 summarizes the model estimates and simple slopes for each of the outcome variables. Short-term effects were statistically significant for parentechild communication and boy/ girlfriend relationship satisfaction and support. Parentechild communication decreased significantly in the comparison group, whereas the Start Strong group showed no such decline at wave 2 (interaction b ¼ .10 [se ¼ .04], p < .05). Start Strong students also reported a statistically significant increase in relationship satisfaction and support from waves 1 to 2, whereas comparison students’ reports did not significantly change over time (groupby-time interaction b ¼ .18 [se ¼ .09], p < .05; and b ¼ .19
S17
[standard error ¼ .09], p < .05, respectively). Effects for the dominance and criticism scales were not statistically significant. Students in the Start Strong schools also reported decreased acceptance of TDV from waves 1 to 2. In contrast, change over time for students in comparison schools was not statistically significant (interaction b ¼ .15 [se ¼ .05], p < .01). Similarly, negative gender stereotypes decreased to a significantly greater extent among Start Strong students (b ¼ .08 [se ¼ .02], p < .01) than comparison students. Rates of change between comparison and intervention conditions were not significantly different for the TDV perpetration and victimization measures. In addition, the rates of change for the overall sample were small and nonsignificant. Groups were not significantly different at baseline on TDV except for electronic victimization, which was significantly lower in Start Strong students (group main effect b ¼ .06 [se¼.02], p < .05). Long-term results Table 2 displays model estimates for wave 1e4 effects for linear LGMs. These models showed fewer statistically significant differences by group. Program effects persisted for acceptance of TDV. Start Strong students decreased in acceptance at a greater rate than comparison students (b ¼ .03 [se ¼ .01], p < .01). Although other constructs showed statistically significant change, their rates of change did not differ significantly between intervention and comparison conditions. As shown by the simple slopes in Table 2, gender stereotypes decreased in both groups; and relationship satisfaction and support both increased over time in program and control students. Estimates from the aggregated follow-up model are provided in Table 3. Baseline values of all outcomes were highly predictive of their respective aggregated postintervention scores. Two outcomes showed group differences across the full study time frame when the requirement of linear change over time was relaxed. Overall levels of gender stereotypes were significantly lower in the program group (b ¼ .08 [se ¼ .03], p < .05). The overall postintervention mean stereotype score was .92 for comparison students and .83 for Start Strong participants. Acceptance was also lower in the intervention group
Table 1 Wave 1 to wave 2 program effects Model estimates, Est (SE)
TDV psychological perpetration TDV physical perpetration TDV electronic perpetration TDV psychological victimization TDV physical victimization TDV electronic victimization Gender stereotypes Acceptance of dating violence Negative consequences Response to anger Communication skills Parentechild communication Relationship satisfaction Relationship support Relationship criticism Relationship dominance
Intercept
Time
.03 .07 .01 .10 .07 .05 .54 .65 2.34 .88 2.03 2.24 2.06 1.91 .05 .11
.02 .01 .01 .02 .01 .01 .03 .02 .01 .04 .05 .10 .06 .05 .00 .00
(.04) (.04) (.03) (.05) (.04) (.03) (.06) (.06) (.06) (.05) (.07) (.06) (.13) (.12) (.05) (.06)
Est (SE) ¼ Estimate (standard error); TDV ¼ teen dating violence. p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
*
(.04) (.02) (.04) (.05) (.02) (.03) (.02) (.03) (.02) (.02)* (.03) (.03)*** (.06) (.06) (.06) (.06)
Simple slopes, Est (SE) Group
Group time
Comparison
Start Strong
.07 .07 .04 .03 .03 .06 .05 .07 .10 .06 .08 .02 .10 .01 .01 .07
.06 .05 .04 .02 .04 .06 .08 .15 .04 .01 .06 .10 .18 .19 .05 .01
.02 .01 .01 .02 .01 .01 .03 .02 .01 .04 .05 .10 .06 .05 .00 .00
.07 .04 .05 .04 .05 .07 .11 .17 .03 .02 .00 .01 .23 .24 .05 .01
(.03) (.04) (.02) (.03) (.03) (.02)* (.06) (.06) (.08) (.03) (.05) (.04) (.09) (.08) (.03) (.04)
(.05) (.04) (.05) (.07) (.03) (.05) (.02)** (.05)** (.04) (.02) (.05) (.04)* (.09)* (.09)* (.09) (.09)
(.04) (.02) (.04) (.05) (.02) (.03) (.02) (.03) (.02) (.02)* (.03) (.03)*** (.06) (.06) (.06) (.06)
(.04) (.02) (.04) (.05) (.03) (.04) (.02)*** (.04)*** (.03) (.02) (.04) (.03) (.06)*** (.06)*** (.06) (.07)
S18
S. Miller et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
Table 2 Wave 1 to wave 4 program effects Model estimates, Est (SE)
TDV psychological perpetration TDV physical perpetration TDV electronic perpetration TDV psychological victimization TDV physical victimization TDV electronic victimization Gender stereotypes Acceptance of dating violence Negative consequences Response to anger Communication skills Parentechild communication Relationship satisfaction Relationship support Relationship criticism Relationship dominance
Intercept
Time
.05 .07 .02 .15 .05 .08 .53 .65 2.35 .09 2.03 2.22 2.12 2.00 .12 .13
.01 .01 .01 .01 .00 .01 .03 .04 .01 .00 .00 .02 .11 .13 .01 .00
(.04) (.04) (.03) (.04) (.04) (.03) (.06) (.06) (.06) (.05) (.06) (.06) (.12) (.11) (.05) (.07)
Simple slopes, Est (SE)
(.01) (.01) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.01) (.01)* (.01)*** (.02) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.02)*** (.03)*** (.02) (.01)
Group
Group time
Comparison
Start Strong
.04 .04 .03 .03 .02 .05 .07 .11 .12 .08 .11 .01 .00 .10 .01 .06
.00 .01 .00 .00 .00 .01 .02 .03 .01 .02 .01 .02 .04 .06 .01 .01
.01 .01 .01 .01 .00 .01 .03 .04 .01 .00 .00 .02 .11 .13 .01 .00
.00 .00 .00 .01 .00 .00 .05 .07 .01 .02 .01 .01 .07 .07 .02 .01
(.04) (.04) (.03) (.03) (.04) (.03) (.07) (.06) (.08) (.04) (.05) (.04) (.10) (.07) (.04) (.07)
(.01) (.01) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.01) (.02) (.01)** (.02) (.02) (.02) (.02) (.04) (.04) (.02) (.02)
(.01) (.01) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.01) (.01)* (.01)*** (.02) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.02)*** (.03)*** (.02) (.01)
(.01) (.01) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.01) (.01)*** (.01)*** (.02) (.01) (.02) (.01) (.03)** (.03)** (.02) (.01)
Est (SE) ¼ Estimate (standard error); TDV ¼ teen dating violence. * p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
(b ¼ .16 [se ¼ .03], p < .01). Means were .77 for comparison students and .61 for Start Strong students. Discussion Results are promising and illustrate that a multicomponent, community-based initiative reduced risk factors predictive of TDV. In the short term, effects were statistically significant for two key relational domains. Parentechild communication decreased among comparison students from the beginning to the end of grade 7 (waves 1 to 2). In contrast, parentechild communication remained stable for Start Strong students. Parentechild communication may decrease in early adolescence as young teens individuate and assert their independence. Yet, parents remain an important relationship as youth enter adolescence and are faced with new developmental challenges [24]. Indeed, positive parentechild ties are linked with lower levels of TDV [8,25]. The fact that parentechild communication
Table 3 Wave 1 to wave 4 program effects, aggregate follow-up model Model estimates, Est (SE)
TDV psychological perpetration TDV physical perpetration TDV electronic perpetration TDV psychological victimization TDV physical victimization TDV electronic victimization Gender stereotypes Acceptance of dating violence Negative consequences Response to anger Communication skills Parentechild communication Relationship satisfaction Relationship support Relationship criticism Relationship dominance
Intercept
Group
Baseline
.05 .06 .03 .18 .00 .09 .10 .22 1.37 .47 1.05 1.12 1.78 1.60 .15 .15
.03 .01 .01 .03 .03 .01 .10 .16 .06 .02 .07 .07 .01 .04 .03 .06
.29 .34 .24 .24 .26 .32 .58 .51 .42 .52 .49 .49 .34 .40 .29 .22
(.05) (.04) (.04) (.06)* (.04) (.04) (.04)* (.04)** (.08)*** (.04***) (.08)*** (.08)*** (.13)*** (.13)*** (.07) (.07)
(.04) (.04) (.04) (.06) (.04) (.04) (.03)* (.03)** (.06) (.04) (.06) (.04) (.09) (.08) (.07) (.05)
Est (SE) ¼ Estimate (standard error); TDV ¼ teen dating violence. * p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
(.04)*** (.03)*** (.04)*** (.03)*** (.03)*** (.03)*** (.02)*** (.02)*** (.02)*** (.02)*** (.02)*** (.02)*** (.03)*** (.03)*** (.03)*** (.03)***
remained stable for Start Strong, yet decreased for comparisons, suggests the initiative may have disrupted the developmentally normative decrease that occurs in early adolescence [24]. Parentechild communication is a potentially malleable domain that can be added to TDV preventive interventions. Short-term findings were also statistically significant for support and satisfaction in boy/girlfriend relationships. Other studies have found that caring relationships are linked with lower TDV perpetration and victimization [26]; reduced drug and alcohol use; and emotional problems [27]. Supportive ties in dating relationships serve as a model for partner selection and healthy relationships later in development [28]. These findings emphasize the need for preventive interventions to focus not only on decreasing TDV but also on promoting healthy relationships. Long-term findings for acceptance of TDV and gender stereotypes persisted longitudinally. These results are particularly encouraging as these factors mediated associations between Safe Dates participation and decreased TDV perpetration and victimization [4]. We speculate that continued and strengthened emphasis on reducing TDV acceptance and gender stereotypes in early adolescence may ultimately decrease TDV over time. Findings were not significant for TDV perpetration and victimization. It may be that TDV rates for more serious violence are lower in early adolescence. Also possible is that measures used here, which were developed for high school students, may not capture behaviors relevant in early adolescence. Studies reveal that early adolescents engage in “poke and push” courtship behaviors to convey dating interest, such as playfully hitting, grabbing, and pushing [29,30]. This poking and pushing may translate to positive responses, thus obscuring possible differences in more aggressive behaviors. The meaning and nature of dating violence among early adolescents may differ. Compared to later adolescence, initial dating affiliations are brief and casual as young teens become comfortable interacting with a potential boyfriend or girlfriend [31]. Additional work is needed with younger teens to understand TDV and healthy relationship concepts and experiences that are salient during early adolescence. This understanding can inform the development and testing of new measures and potential modification of existing measures,
S. Miller et al. / Journal of Adolescent Health 56 (2015) S14eS19
to ensure that instruments are developmentally relevant for young teens. This study contains limitations. As with other evaluations of multicomponent initiatives, we cannot identify specific components that led to statistically significant findings. In addition, the consent rates were somewhat low, although they are well within the range of similar school-based studies [32]. Obtaining active consent within school settings can be challenging, particularly in older grades where parents are less involved. A few of the measures displayed somewhat low internal consistency, which may have been due to being developed for older adolescents. Additionally, only 3 of the 11 grantees were included in the evaluation, and thus, results cannot generalize to all the sites. We still, however, have the asset of a community-based multifaceted initiative with enough stability to assess the sites that were able to be compared. In addition, implementation varied for components other than the Safe Dates curriculum. However, regardless of the implementation variations, the community-based initiative produced these differences. Community-driven initiatives such as Start Strong may promote greater community buy-in from key stakeholders. At the same time, imposing program components may impede ownership by key stakeholders and ultimately decrease the likelihood of sustainability once external funding has ended. This challenge is noteworthy in the TDV field where evidence-based approaches are few in number. Despite these limitations, this study is noteworthy in its longitudinal evaluation of a multicomponent initiative targeting 11- to 14-year-olds. The Start Strong initiative is innovative in its focus on early adolescence, which is a critical period in the transition to early dating [33]. The results inform future intervention efforts and underscore the need for further study of middle school students. Acknowledgments We appreciate the support of the sites and schools that participated in the evaluation. We also want to thank Futures Without Violence, California Blue Shield Foundation, and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation for their ongoing efforts throughout the project. References [1] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Understanding teen dating violence fact sheet. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2012. [2] Teten AL, Ball B, Valle LA, et al. Considerations for the definition, measurement, consequences, and prevention of dating violence victimization among adolescent girls. J Womens Health (Larchmt) 2009;18:923e7. [3] Exner-Cortens D, Eckenrode J, Rothman E. Longitudinal associations between teen dating violence victimization and adverse health outcomes. Pediatrics 2013;131:71e8. [4] Foshee VA, Bauman KE, Ennett ST, et al. Assessing the effects of the dating violence prevention program “safe dates” using random coefficient regression modeling. Prev Sci 2005;6:245e58. [5] Wolfe DA, Crooks C, Jaffe P, et al. A school-based program to prevent adolescent dating violence: A cluster randomized trial. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2009;163:692e9. [6] Taylor BG, Stein ND, Mumford EA, Woods D. Shifting boundaries: An experimental evaluation of a dating violence prevention program in middle schools. Prev Sci 2013;14:64e76. [7] Foshee VA, McNaughton Reyes HL, Ennett ST, et al. Assessing the effects of Families for Safe Dates, a family-based teen dating abuse prevention program. J Adolesc Health 2012;51:349e56.
S19
[8] Miller E, Tancredi DJ, McCauley HL, et al. One-year follow-up of a coachdelivered dating violence prevention program: A cluster randomized controlled trial. Am J Prev Med 2013;45:108e12. [9] Simon TR, Miller S, Gorman-Smith D, et al. Physical dating violence norms and behavior among sixth-grade students from four U.S. sites. J Early Adolesc 2010;30:395e409. [10] Furman W, Rose AJ. Friendships, romantic relationships, and other dyadic peer relationships in childhood and adolescence: A unified relational perspective. In: Lerner R, Lamb ME, Coll CG, eds. The handbook of child psychology and developmental science. 7th edition, Vol. 3. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2013. [11] Tharp AP, Maffini MV, Hunt PA, et al. Bisphenol A alters the development of the rhesus monkey mammary gland. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2012;109: 8190e5. [12] O’Leary KD, Slep AM. Prevention of partner violence by focusing on behaviors of both young males and females. Prev Sci 2012;13:329e39. [13] Hawkins JD, Kosterman R, Catalano RF, et al. Effects of social development intervention in childhood 15 years later. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008; 162:1133e41. [14] Multisite Violence Prevention Project. The Multisite Violence Prevention Project: Impact of a Universal School-Based Violence Prevention Program on Social-Cognitive Outcomes. Prevention Science 2008;9:231e44. [15] Multisite Violence Prevention Project. The ecological effects of universal and selective violence prevention programs for middle school students: A randomized trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 2009;77: 526e42. [16] Foshee VA, Linder GF, Bauman KE, et al. The Safe Dates Project: Theoretical basis, evaluation design, and selected baseline findings. Am J Prev Med 1996;12(5 Suppl):39e47. [17] National Registry of Evidence-Based Programs and Practices. Homepage: National Registry of Evidence-based Programs and Practices. Available at: http://nrepp.samhsa.gov/; 2013. Accessed January 29, 2014. [18] Glauber A. Tween and teen dating violence and abuse study. Available at: http://www.breakthecycle.org/sites/default/files/pdf/survey-lina-tweens2008.pdf; 2008. Accessed October 17, 2014. [19] Mitchell KJ, Jones LM. Youth Internet Safety (YISS) Study: Methodology report. Durham, NH: University of New Hampshire, Crimes Against Children Research Center; 2011. [20] Sales JM, Milhausen RR, Wingood GM, et al. Validation of a ParentAdolescent Communication Scale for use in STD/HIV prevention interventions. Health Educ Behav 2008;35:332e45. [21] Furman W, Buhrmester D. Children’s perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Dev Psychol 1985;21:1016e22. [22] Little R, Rubin D. Statistical analysis with missing data. 2nd edition. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley; 2002. [23] Schafer JL, Graham JW. Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychol Methods 2002;7:147e77. [24] Keijsers L, Poulin F. Developmental changes in parent-child communication throughout adolescence. Dev Psychol 2013;49:2301e8. [25] Lohman BJ, Neppl TK, Senia JM, Schofield TJ. Understanding adolescent and family influences on intimate partner psychological violence during emerging adulthood and adulthood. J Youth Adolesc 2013;42: 500e17. [26] Orpinas P, Hsieh HL, Song X, et al. Trajectories of physical dating violence from middle to high school: Association with relationship quality and acceptability of aggression. J Youth Adolescence 2013;42:551e65. [27] Collins WA, Welsh DP, Furman W. Adolescent romantic relationships. Annu Rev Psychol 2009;60:631e52. [28] Manning WD, Giordano PC, Longmore MA. Adolescent dating relationships: Implications for understanding adult unions. Available at: http:// www.soc.duke.edu/wefc/Unions/Docs/Manning.pdf; 2008. Accessed January 29, 2014. [29] Maccoby EE. The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press; 1998. [30] Pelligrini AD, Long JD. A sexual selection theory longitudinal analysis of sexual segregation and integration in early adolescence. J Exp Child Psychol 2003;85:257e78. [31] Meier A, Allen G. Romantic relationships from adolescence to young adulthood: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Sociol Q 2009;50:308e35. [32] Blom-Hoffman J, Leff SS, Franko DL, et al. Consent procedures and participation rates in school-based intervention and prevention research: Using a multi-component, partnership-based approach to recruit participants. School Ment Health 2009;1:3e15. [33] Furman W, Low S, Ho MJ. Romantic experience and psychosocial adjustment in middle adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2009;38: 75e90.