Ideology and the promotion of social change

Ideology and the promotion of social change

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Ideology and the promotion of social change Julia C Becker Most models predicting collective ...

465KB Sizes 1 Downloads 99 Views

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect Ideology and the promotion of social change Julia C Becker Most models predicting collective action for social change have not considered the role of ideology. Recent research suggests, however, that ideology is a key variable in predicting social change efforts by illustrating that conservatives are more likely to resist whereas liberals are more likely to promote social change. In this review, I bring two lines of research together showing that not ony liberals but also right-wing populists are interested in collective action for social change. I introduce an integrative framework that endeavors to clarify the relation between ideology and social change by taking the content of socio-political issues into account. I propose that the distinction between progressive and reactionary social change can help to connect different social change intentions with its underlying ideological antecedents. Address University of Osnabrueck, Seminarstraße 20, 49074 Osnabrueck, Germany Corresponding author: Becker, Julia C ([email protected])

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2019, 34:6–11 This review comes from a themed issue on Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies* Edited by John Jost, Eran Halperin and Kristin Laurin

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.10.005 2352-1546/ã 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Political ideology comprises attitudes and values about the proper goals of society and how they should be achieved [1– 3,4]. Thus, political ideologies are motivated social cognitions [5]. Central elements are attitudes toward social stability (versus social change) and toward social hierarchy (versus social equality). Importantly for the present review, people’s preferred political ideology can morally compel them to engage in political activism that promotes social change or upholds the status quo [6]. In this review, I first present evidence showing that liberals are more likely to promote social change compared to conservatives. Second, I review work challenging the assumption that conservatives always resist change and connect the literature on ideology and social change with work on right-wing extremism and propose that a distinction between progressive and reactionary social Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11

change can help to connect different social change intentions with their underlying ideological antecedents. Finally, I discuss open questions and provide directions for future work.

Liberals promote social change whereas conservatives promote system stability Until recently, psychologists did not consider political ideology as unique predictor of collective action for social change (e.g. Refs. [7–10], for exceptions see, for example, [11]). One possible reason for not considering political ideology in prominent models of collective action is that most researchers studied collective action for progressive social change which aims at a more open, tolerant and egalitarian society and as action that challenges established systems of authority. For instance, left-wing political orientation predicted the intention to engage in Gezi park protests [12], and liberal ideology predicted participation in the Occupy Wall Street demonstrations [13]. Thus, progressive social change is the default in most research on collective action. In contrast, research on the role of conservative ideology focused on system maintenance, defense of the existing social order, and resistance to social change (e.g. Refs. [6,14]). Indeed, a number of authors argued and provided empirical support for the notion that a core feature of political conservatism is the resistance to social change, whereas a core feature of political liberalism is the promotion of social change (e. g. Refs. [2,15–20]). According to Jost et al., these findings can be explained by differences among liberals and conservatives in epistemic needs (e.g. need to reduce uncertainty), existential needs (e.g. need for security) and relational needs (e.g. need for shared reality, [19]). For instance, conservatives have stronger psychological needs to maintain what is familiar and known and to reject the unknown, risky and uncertain prospects of social change [19,21]. Recently, Jost et al. [6] explicitly incorporated ideology into a model of collective action by differentiating individuals endorsing or rejecting the system and by considering system-supporting versus system-challenging collective action [6,14]. System-supporting collective action refers to protests aiming at maintaining the status quo, whereas system challenging collective action refers to protests aiming at changing established systems of inequality (see Figure 1). In two empirical tests, Osborne et al. [14] showed that system justification is negatively associated with system-challenging collective action, but positively associated with system-supporting collective action for members of groups of low and high status. The well-established predictors of collective action (group www.sciencedirect.com

Ideology and the promotion of social change Becker 7

Figure 1

Perceived Group Efficacy

Group-Based Anger

Group Identification

LS

SystemChallenging Protest (vs. Inaction)

LS

HS HS

HS

Perceived Injustice Epistemic Needs Existential Needs

LS

System Justification

SystemSupporting Protest (vs. Inaction)

System-Based Anger

Relational Needs Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

A schematic illustration of a model of system-challenging and system-supporting protest. Note: HS = high status (or advantaged) group members; LS = low-status (or disadvantaged) group members. This figure is based on an original figure from Jost et al. ([6], Figure 7).

identification, group-based injustice, group-based anger, and system-based dissatisfaction/anger) served as mediators of these relationships.

society (see Figure 2). Next, I review recent research on ideology and the promotion of progressive social change, before I focus on ideology and the promotion of reactionary social change.

Progressive versus reactionary social change So far, the reviewed literature suggests that liberals and conservatives have different needs, which cause a stronger inclination toward social change among liberals, and a stronger inclination to support the status quo among conservatives. This might result in the impression that social change is always progressive, leading to more equality in society. When we look at the world politics, however, we do not see only left-wing movements that advocate for social change. In fact, in contemporary Europe, the number of radical right-wing populist movements is on the rise [22]. These movements are not at all interested in system stability, but in social change aiming at changing society into a more hierarchical and less equal one, for instance, by restricting immigration, and fostering law and order politics [22]. Therefore, a distinction between progressive versus reactionary social change seems warranted to highlight that system rejection and social change can also be promoted by right-wing movements. In this review, I define progressive social change as change aiming at establishing more social equality in society, whereas reactionary social change as change aiming at fostering more social inequality in www.sciencedirect.com

Progressive social change and its ideological antecedents In many studies, political ideology is conceptualized as two separate but related constructs: social dominance orientation (SDO) and right-wing authoritarianism (RWA, e.g. Ref. [16]). SDO reflects a competition-driven Figure 2

Progressive social change Political Ideology (right-left) Reactionary social change Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

Ideology predicts progressive versus reactionary social change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11

8 Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies*

motivation for group-based dominance and hierarchy [16,23], and correlates with opposition to equality as well as with prejudice toward those aiming to attenuate social hierarchy [24,25]. RWA expresses a threat-driven motivation for collective security and ingroup cohesion [16]. Further, it presents adherence to established social conventions, deference to established authorities and aggression against norm-deviants [26]. The underlying psychological needs, traits and motives reflect needs for certainty, security and order, which is associated with a preference for maintaining the status quo as opposed to support for protest [27]. Recent work illustrated that low levels of SDO predict progressive political engagement (e.g. Refs. [28–33], see also Ref. [34] for a more refined analysis). Moreover, researchers found a negative relationship between RWA [35], SDO [36] and support of progressive protest. Similarly, work in the context of more novel social movements such as #fridaysforfuture and pro-environmental action illustrated that RWA and SDO predict lower willingness to act for climate change [37–39]; see also Ref. [40] for the reverse causal order). Furthermore, system justification theorists argue that people are motivated to defend the status quo and systems on which they depend because it serves epistemic motives to reduce uncertainty, existential motives to reduce threat and relational motive to share reality with the mainstream [41,42]. In line with this, low levels of system justification ([15]; see also Ref. [43–46], and van Stekelenburg and Klandermans for examining the role of trust in politicians [47]) but some degree of system confidence [48] predicted political engagement for progressive social change (see also Ref. [49] for the reversed causal order). Some further variables representing inequality-related ideology predicted political engagement for progressive social change: low levels of paternalism [50], low levels of hope for future ingroup status change [51], low levels of masculine honor ideology [52], and low levels of postcolonial beliefs [53]. In a nutshell, there is a lot of evidence showing that liberal ideologies (e.g. left-wing political orientation, low levels of SDO and RWA, low levels in system justification) promote collective action for progressive social change. In addition to this, some first studies illustrate that under certain conditions, conservatives become less resistant against progressive social change [54–56].

Reactionary social change and its ideological antecedents Reactionary politics reflect a preference for political affairs that have been overcome, combining a resentful affectivity with the desire to return to the past [57]. In its contemporary form, it is often used to describe the discourse of right-wing populist parties [58,59,60]. Often, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11

right-wing movements get more support in times of crisis (e.g. Ref. [61], but see Ref. [62]). For instance, an analysis of social movements in France between 1882 and 1980 illustrated that threat (assessed as long-term episodes of economic deprivation) increased the presence of reactionary movements [63]. Research in the last two years provides empirical support for the argument that conservatives and liberals can both resist and accept societal changes. The resistance or acceptance depends on their approval of the existing societal status quo on a given sociopolitical issue [4]. Recently, Caricati [64] presented evidence of decreased system justification among extreme conservatives. He illustrates a quadratic and negative relationship showing that system justification is highest in the middle and decreases at the left and right extremes of the political continuum. This finding is in line with Thorisdottir et al. showing that ideological extremity on the left and right was associated with greater openness to new experiences [65]. Similarly, Choma et al. [66] illustrated that RWA and SDO related positively to collective action aiming at addressing societal moral breakdown, but negatively to collective action targeting at equalizing race relations or fighting climate change (see also Refs. [67,68]). These studies support the idea that individuals are more inclined to participate in collective action in domains that align with their existing values or political affinity [66,4,68]. Relatedly, Mikolajczak and Becker [69] exposed liberals and conservatives to potential changes in the tax reform and in the healthcare system and then asked whether they intent to engage in collective action to protest against these changes. They manipulated whether the reason for the reform was based on a violation of the merit, equality or need rule of fairness. Whereas conservatives and liberals were equally interested in engaging in collective action for social change across conditions, conservatives were more likely to endorse protest when the merit rule of fairness was violated, whereas liberals were more likely to endorse protest when the need and equality rules were at stake [69]. Some research also illustrated that a right-wing ideological variables (here political orientation, [70]) and SDO [25] predict actions to counter progressive movements such as repression of Gezi park protests [70] and anti-immigrant action to sustain group-based hierarchy [25]. In sum, although conservatives have a stronger inclination to support the status quo (see Figure 1, [6]), those in the extreme right do not necessarily advocate for system stability but aim to initiate social change in a reactionary direction (see Figure 2).

Conclusion and directions for future research In this review, I highlighted two observations that are contradictory at first sight: on the one hand, a lot of evidence suggests that liberals compared to conservatives www.sciencedirect.com

Ideology and the promotion of social change Becker 9

are more supportive of social change in general [6]. On the other hand, individuals endorsing right-wing ideology reject the system [64] and promote reactionary social change [66]. In order to reconcile these two observations, I proposed a distinction between progressive and reactionary social change and reviewed evidence showing that individuals endorsing left-wing ideology (e.g. liberals, individuals rejecting SDO and RWA) are likely to promote progressive social change, whereas individuals endorsing right-wing ideology (e.g. right-wing populism, SDO and RWA) are likely to promote reactionary social change. Thus, there seems to be a distinction between ‘moderate conservatives’ who support the status quo and right-wing extremists who support reactionary social change. In the following, I provide some directions for future research. First, we need to clarify the distinction between the liberalism-conservatism and left-right dimensions. Most work in the US use the liberal-conservative dimension [21], whereas in many European countries, the left-right dimension or the distinction between social and economic conservatism is more meaningful (e.g. Ref. [65]). As illustrated, right-wing extremists do not want ‘to conserve’ the status quo, they want radical reactionary change and they cannot be identified in research projects that use the liberalconservative dimension to assess political ideology. For instance, historical analyses show that in Germany, the meaning of what is ‘right’ and what is ‘left’ changed profoundly since the French revolution [71]. Thus, more research is necessary to examine psychological and ideological differences and similarities among ‘extreme conservatism’ and ‘right-wing extremism’. Related to this, it is likely that the strength of ideology is crucial in predicting the promotion of social change. Individuals for whom their ideology is central (compared to those who are more uncertain about their political orientation) should be more likely to identify with their ideological ingroup and this politicized identity should predict the promotion of social change [72– 74]. Thus, future work could test whether strength of ideology endorsement predicts politicized identification which in turn promotes progressive versus reactionary social change. Second, although recent research considered ideology in models of collective action for social change, it is still unclear whether there are ideological differences in group efficacy, group-based emotions, and moral convictions. Following Mikolajczak and Becker [69] who illustrated ideological differences in injustice appraisals, future research could examine a) whether left-wing and rightwing individuals differ in other established predictors of collective action and b) whether the experience of group efficacy, group identification, group-based emotions and moral convictions motivate left-wing and right-wing individuals to the same extend to engage in collective action for social change. www.sciencedirect.com

Conflict of interest statement Nothing declared.

References and recommended reading Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:  of special interest  of outstanding interest 1.

Jost JT: The end of the end of ideology. Am Psychol 2006, 61:651-670.

2.

Jost JT: Ideological asymmetries and the essence of political psychology. Polit Psychol 2017, 38:167-208.

3.

Erikson RS, Tedin KL: American Public Opinion. Longman; 2003.

4. 

Proch J, Elad-Strenger J, Kessler T: Liberalism and conservatism, for a change! Rethinking the association between political orientation and relation to societal change. Polit Psychol 2019, 40:877-903 The authors show that both conservatives and liberals can resist and accept societal changes. They show that it is important to consider to which degree individuals approve or disapprove of the status quo on a sociopolitical issue.

5.

Jost JT, Pelham BW, Sheldon O, Sullivan BN: Social inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the system: evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvantaged. Eur J Soc Psychol 2003, 33:13-36.

6. 

Jost JT, Becker J, Osborne D, Badaan V: Missing in (Collective) action: ideology, system justification, and the motivational antecedents of two types of protest behavior. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2017, 26:99-108 The authors take a system-justification perspective and specify when individuals engage in system-challenging protest and when in systemsupporting protest.

7.

Becker JC, Tausch N: A dynamic model of engagement in normative and non-normative collective action: psychological antecedents, consequences, and barriers. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 2015, 26:43-92.

8.

Van Zomeren M, Spears R, Leach CW: Exploring psychological mechanisms of collective action: does relevance of group identity influence how people cope with collective disadvantage? Br J Soc Psychol 2008, 47:353-372.

9.

van Zomeren M, Kutlaca M, Turner-Zwinkels F: Integrating who “we” are with what “we” (Will not) stand for: a further extension of the social identity model of collective action. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 2018, 29:122-160.

10. van Zomeren M: Toward a cultural psychology of collective action: Just how “core” are the core motivations for collective action? J Pacific Rim Psychol 2019, 13. 11. Klandermans B, van Stekelenburg J: Social Movements and the Dynamics of Collective Action. 2013. 12. Stewart AL, Leach CW, Bilali R, C¸elik AB, Cidam A: Explaining  different orientations to the 2013 Gezi Park demonstrations in Istanbul, Turkey. Br J Soc Psychol 2019. 01610. 13. Langer M, Jost JT, Bonneau R, Metzger MM, Noorbaloochi S,  Penfold-Brown D: Digital dissent: an analysis of the motivational contents of tweets from an Occupy Wall Street demonstration. Motiv Sci 2019, 5:14-34. 14. Osborne D, Jost JT, Becker JC, Badaan V, Sibley CG: Protesting  to challenge or defend the system? A system justification perspective on collective action. Eur J Soc Psychol 2019, 49:244-269 The authors show that system justification Is negatively related with system-challenging collective action, but positively associated with system-supporting collective action. 15. Badaan V, Jost JT, Osborne D, Sibley CG, Ungaretti J, Etchezahar E, Hennes EP: Social protest and its discontents. Contention 2018, 6:1-22. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11

10 Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies*

16. Duckitt J: Differential effects of right wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation on outgroup attitudes and their mediation by threat from and competitiveness to outgroups. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2006, 32:684-696.

33. Thomas EF, Smith LGE, McGarty C, Reese G, Kende A, Bliuc AM, Curtin N, Spears R: When and how social movements mobilize action within and across nations to promote solidarity with refugees. Eur J Soc Psychol 2019, 49:213-229.

17. Jost JT, Glaser J, Kruglanski AW, Sulloway FJ: Exceptions that prove the rule - using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies: Reply to Greenberg and Jonas. Psychol Bull 2003, 129:383-393.

34. Lucas BJ, Kteily NS: (Anti-)egalitarianism differentially predicts empathy for members of advantaged versus disadvantaged groups. J Pers Soc Psychol 2018, 114:665-692.

18. van der Toorn J, Jost JT, Packer DJ, Noorbaloochi S, Van Bavel JJ: In defense of tradition: religiosity, conservatism, and opposition to same-sex marriage in North America. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2017, 43:1455-1468. 19. Jost JT, Federico CM, Napier JL: Political Ideology: its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annu Rev Psychol 2009, 60:307-337.

35. Weiner E, Federico CM: Authoritarianism, institutional confidence, and willingness to engage in collective action: a multinational analysis. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2017, 43:392-406. 36. Kearns EM, Asal V, Walsh JI, Federico C, Lemieux AF: Political action as a function of grievances, risk, and social identity: an experimental approach. Stud Confl Terror 2018, 0:1-18. 37. Jylha¨ KM, Akrami N: Social dominance orientation and climate change denial: The role of dominance and system justification. Pers Individ Dif 2015, 86:108-111.

20. Kahn DT, Tagar MR, Halperin E, Ba¨ckstro¨m M, Vitriol JA, Liberman V: If they can’t change, why support change? Implicit theories about groups, social dominance orientation and political identity. J Soc Polit Psychol 2018, 6:151-173.

38. Jylha¨ KM, Cantal C, Akrami N, Milfont TL: Denial of anthropogenic climate change: Social dominance orientation helps explain the conservative male effect in Brazil and Sweden. Pers Individ Dif 2016, 98:184-187.

21. Jost JT, Napier JL, Thorisdottir H, Gosling SD, Palfai TP, Ostafin B: Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2007, 33:989-1007.

39. Stanley SK, Wilson MS, Sibley CG, Milfont TL: Dimensions of social dominance and their associations with environmentalism. Pers Individ Dif 2017, 107:228-236.

22. Muis J, Immerzeel T: Causes and consequences of the rise of populist radical right parties and movements in Europe. Curr Sociol 2017, 65:909-930.

40. Stanley SK, Milfont TL, Wilson MS, Sibley CG: The influence of social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism on environmentalism: a five-year cross-lagged analysis. PLoS One 2019, 14:e0219067.

23. Ho AK, Sidanius J, Kteily N, Sheehy-Skeffington J, Pratto F, Henkel KE, Foels R, Stewart AL: The nature of social dominance orientation: theorizing and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality using the new SDO7 scale. J Pers Soc Psychol 2015, 109:1003-1028.

41. Jost JT, Gaucher D, Stern C: “The world isn’t fair”: a system justification perspective on social stratification and inequality. APA Handb Personal Soc Psychol Vol 2 Gr Process 2014, 2:317340.

24. Crawford JT, Mallinas SR, Furman BJ: The balanced ideological antipathy model: explaining the effects of ideological attitudes on inter-group antipathy across the political spectrum. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2015, 41:1607-1622. 25. Kunst JR, Fischer R, Sidanius J, Thomsen L: Preferences for group dominance track and mediate the effects of macro-level social inequality and violence across societies. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2017, 114:5407-5412. 26. Altemeyer B: Enemies of Freedom: Understanding Right-wing Authoritarianism. Jossey-Bass; 1988. 27. Carney DR, Jost JT, Gosling SD, Potter J: The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Polit Psychol 2008, 29:807-840. 28. Hoskin RE, Thomas EF, McGarty C: Transnational contact and challenging global poverty: Intergroup contact intensifies (the right kind of) social identities to promote solidarity-based collective action for those low in social dominance. J Theor Soc Psychol 2019, 3:23-34. 29. Milfont TL, Bain PG, Kashima Y, Corral-Verdugo V, Pasquali C, Johansson LO, Guan Y, Gouveia VV, Garðarsdo´ttir RB, Doron G et al.: On the relation between social dominance orientation and environmentalism: A 25-nation study. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2018, 9:802-814. 30. Rodriguez-Bailon R, Bratanova B, Willis GB, Lopez-Rodriguez L, Sturrock A, Loughnan S: Social class and ideologies of inequality: how they uphold unequal societies. J Soc Issues 2017, 73:99-116. 31. Stewart AL, Tran J: Protesting Racial hierarchy: testing a social dominance theory model of collective action among White Americans. J Soc Issues 2018, 74:299-316. 32. Stewart AL, Pratto F, Bou Zeineddine F, Sweetman J, Eicher V, Licata L, Morselli D, Saab R, Aiello A, Chryssochoou X et al.: International support for the Arab uprisings: Understanding sympathetic collective action using theories of social dominance and social identity. Gr Process Intergr Relations 2016, 19:6-26. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11

42. Jost JT, Langer M, Badaan V, Azevedo F, Etchezahar E, Ungaretti J, Hennes EP: Ideology and the limits of self-interest: system justification motivation and conservative advantages in mass politics. Transl Issues Psychol Sci 2017, 3:e1-e26. 43. Jost JT, Badaan V, Goudarzi S, Hoffarth M, Mogami M: The future of system justification theory. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:382-392. 44. Friesen JP, Laurin K, Shepherd S, Gaucher D, Kay AC: System justification: experimental evidence, its contextual nature, and implications for social change. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:315-339. 45. Jost JT: A quarter century of system justification theory: questions, answers, criticisms, and societal applications. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:263-314. 46. Osborne D, Sengupta NK, Sibley CG: System justification theory at 25: evaluating a paradigm shift in psychology and looking towards the future. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:340-361. 47. van Stekelenburg J, Klandermans B: In politics we trust . . . or not? Trusting and distrusting demonstrators compared. Polit Psychol 2018, 39:775-792. 48. Cichocka A, Go´rska P, Jost JT, Sutton RM, Bilewicz M: What  inverted u can do for your country: a curvilinear relationship between confidence in the social system and political engagement. J Pers Soc Psychol 2018, 115:883-902. 49. Saguy T, Szekeres H: Changing minds via collective action: exposure to the 2017 Women’s March predicts decrease in (some) men’s gender system justification over time. Gr Process Intergr Relations 2018, 21:678-689. 50. Becker JC, Ksenofontov I, Siem B, Love A: Antecedents and consequences of autonomy- and dependency-oriented help toward refugees. Eur J Soc Psychol 2019, 49:831-838. 51. Owuamalam CK, Rubin M, Spears R: Revisiting 25 years of system motivation explanation for system justification from the perspective of social identity model of system attitudes. Br J Soc Psychol 2019, 58:362-381. 52. Travaglino GA, Abrams D, de Moura GR: Men of honor don’t talk: the relationship between masculine honor and social activism against criminal organizations in Italy. Polit Psychol 2016, 37:183-199. www.sciencedirect.com

Ideology and the promotion of social change Becker 11

53. Osborne D, Yogeeswaran K, Sibley CG: Culture-specific ideologies undermine collective action support: examining the legitimizing effects of postcolonial belief systems. Gr Process Intergr Relations 2017, 20:333-349.

65. Thorisdottir H, Jost JT, Liviatan I, Shrout PE: Psychological needs and values underlying left-right political orientation: crossnational evidence from Eastern and Western Europe. Public Opin Q 2007, 71:175-203.

54. Milfont TL, Davies CL, Wilson MS: The moral foundations of environmentalism: care- and fairness-based morality interact with political liberalism to predict pro-environmental actions. Soc Psychol Bull 2019, 14.

66. Choma B, Hodson G, Jagayat A, Hoffarth MR: Right-wing  Ideology as a predictor of collective action: a test across four political issue domains. Polit Psychol 2019, 0 The authors show that RWA and SDO are both related positively to collective action targeting societal moral breakdown but negatively to collective action aimed at equalizing race relations or fighting climate change.

55. Gutting RS: Contentious activities, disrespectful protesters: effect of protest context on protest support and mobilization across ideology and authoritarianism. Polit Behav 2019 http:// dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11109-018-09523-8. in press. 56. Napier JL, Huang J, Vonasch AJ, Bargh JA: Superheroes for change: physical safety promotes socially (but not economically) progressive attitudes among conservatives. Eur J Soc Psychol 2018, 48:187-195. 57. Capelos T, Katsanidou A, Demertzis N: Epistήmh kai Koinvnίa: Epiueώrhsh Politikή§ kai Huikή§ Qevrίa§. 2017. 58. Capelos T, Katsanidou A: Reactionary politics: explaining the  psychological roots of anti preferences in European integration and immigration debates. Polit Psychol 2018, 39:1271-1288.

67. Milesi P, Alberici AI: Pluralistic morality and collective action: the role of moral foundations. Gr Process Intergr Relations 2018, 21:235-256. 68. Saeri AK, Iyer A, Louis WR: Right-wing authoritarianism and social dominance orientation predict outsiders’ responses to an external group conflict: implications for identification, anger, and collective action. Anal Soc Issues Public Policy 2015, 15:303-332.

59. Fraser N: Progressive Neoliberalism Versus Reactionary Populism: a Hobson’s Choice. Gt Regres; 2017.

69. Mikolajczak G, Becker JC: What is (un)fair? Political ideology  and collective action. J Soc Polit Psychol 2019, 7:810-829 The authors show that conservatives are more likely to engage in collective action when the merit rule of fairness is violated whereas liberals are more likely to engage in collective action when the equality and need rules are violated.

60. Gervais BT, Morris IL: Reactionary Republicanism. How the Tea Party in the House Paved the way for Traump’s Victory. Oxford University Press; 2018.

70. Akkuş B, Postmes T, Stroebe K, Baray G: Cultures of conflict: protests, violent repression, and community values. Br J Soc Psychol 2019 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12328. in press.

61. van Prooijen JW, Krouwel APM, Emmer J: Ideological responses to the EU refugee crisis: the left, the right, and the extremes. Soc Psychol Personal Sci 2018, 9:143-150.

71. Solty I: links/rechts. In Historisch-kritisches Wo¨rterbuch des Marxismus. Edited by Haug WF, Haug F, Jehle P, Ku¨ttl W. Argument-Verlag; 2015:1153-1168.

62. Hartwich L, Becker JC: Exposure to neoliberalism increases resentment of the elite via feelings of anomie and negative psychological reactions. J Soc Issues 2019, 75:113-133.

72. Drury J, Reicher S: The conservative crowd? How participation in collective events transforms participants’ understandings of collective action. In From Rage to Revolution: Models of Dramatic Social Change. Edited by Wagoner B, Moghadam F, Valsiner J. Cambridge University Press; 2018:11-28.

63. Varaine S: Bad times are not good times for revolutions:  Collective deprivation and the mobilization level of French radical movements (1882–1980). J Commun Appl Soc Psychol 2018, 28:258-271. 64. Caricati L: Evidence of decreased system justification among  extreme conservatives in non-American samples. J Soc Psychol 2019, 4545 The author illustrates a quadratic and negative relationship showing that system justification is highest in the middle and decreases at the left and right extremes of the political continuum.

www.sciencedirect.com

73. Drury J, Reicher S: Collective action and psychological change: the emergence of new social identities. Br J Soc Psychol 2000, 39:579-604. 74. Stu¨rmer S, Simon B: The role of collective identification in social movement participation: a panel study in the context of the German gay movement. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 2004, 30:263-277.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:6–11