Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Learning and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif
Past and future academic experiences are related with present scholastic achievement when intelligence is controlled Gina C. Lemos a,⁎, Francisco J. Abad b, Leandro S. Almeida a, Roberto Colom b a b
Universidade do Minho, Portugal Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history: Received 17 May 2013 Received in revised form 7 January 2014 Accepted 24 January 2014 Keywords: Cognitive ability Academic achievement Academic failure Academic aspirations
a b s t r a c t Here the simultaneous relationships among cognitive ability (CA), past academic failure (PAF), future academic aspirations (FAA), and present scholastic achievement (PSA) were investigated. For addressing these rarely considered relations, two independent representative samples comprising 2796 students were analyzed; the first sample (young adolescents) included 1695 students from the third cycle of elementary school, whereas the second sample (old adolescents) comprised 1101 students from secondary school. SEM (structural equation model) analyses were computed and the main findings revealed that (1) CA, PAF, and FAA predict PSA, (2) CA is the best predictor of PSA, and (3) excluding PAF and FAA from the final SEM model produces a substantial reduction in the achieved predictive validity, especially for Language. © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction Understanding the causes underlying the observed widespread differences in scholastic achievement is a basic goal of scientific research, and it is also relevant for society (Chen & Kaplan, 2003; Haveman & Smeeding, 2006; Heaven & Ciarrochi, 2012; Kao & Thompson, 2003; Lee, Hill, & Hawkins, 2012; Phillipson & Phillipson, 2012; Porter, 2002; Wilson, 2001). Pioneering research frameworks have nominated large sets of cognitive and non-cognitive relevant factors (Webb, 1915). In this regard, Harris (1940) and Cattell (1965) highlighted three basic domains: (i) cognitive ability, (ii) effort (drive or degree of motivation), and (iii) personal, economic, social, and academic circumstances. Cognitive ability is a well established predictor of scholastic achievement (Colom & Flores-Mendoza, 2007; Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007; Jensen, 1998a; Laidra, Pillmann, & Allik, 2007; Neisser et al., 1996; Primi, Ferrão, & Almeida, 2010) with correlations ranging from .30 to .70 (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005; Deary et al., 2007; Jensen, 1998a,b; Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2004; Kyttälä & Lehto, 2008; Rosander, Bäckström, & Stenberg, 2011; Taub, Keith, Floyd, & Mcgrew, 2008), but non-cognitive factors also play a role (Bratko, Chamorro-Premuzic, & Saks, 2006; Conard, 2006; Farsides & Woodfield, 2003; Freiberger, Steinmayr, & Spinath, 2012; Furnham & Chamorro-Premuzic, 2004; Furnham, Chamorro-Premuzic, & McDougall, 2003; Gilles & Bailleux, 2001; Kane & Brand, 2006; Kappe & van der Flier, 2012; Noftle & Robins, 2007; O'Conner & Paunonen, 2007; Poropat, 2009). However, evidence
⁎ Corresponding author at: Centro de Investigação em Educação, Universidade do Minho, Campus de Gualtar, 4710-057 Braga, Portugal. Tel.: +351 927433844. E-mail address:
[email protected] (G.C. Lemos). 1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.01.004
regarding further factors such as past academic failure and future academic aspirations is hardly considered. In this respect, Bandura (1986, 1997) suggested that academic aspirations (the desired scholar outcome or how much schooling is wanted) and expectations (the most likely scholar outcome pursued or how much schooling is expected) can be relevant predictors of present and future educational attainment and occupational status in adulthood (Beal & Crockett, 2010; Feliciano & Rumbaut, 2005; Kao & Thompson, 2003; MacLeod, 1995). It is generally accepted that a better previous academic background leads to better chances of success in present scholastic achievement. Recent research shows that educational aspirations are associated with actual achievement and are an important predictor of achievement in school and beyond (Rothon, Arephin, Klineberg, et al., 2011). Even after controlling for a number of variables, such as prior achievement, educational aspirations are still strongly related with students' achievement (Rothon et al., 2011). Academic aspirations and expectations are not stable throughout life and can be shaped and influenced, positively and negatively, by a wide constellation of factors (e.g. gender, age, ethnicity, sexual orientation, disability, socioeconomic status, religion, and other external factors such as peers, parents and teachers; e.g. Black, 2002; Cheng & Starks, 2002; Danziger & Eden, 2007; Goldstein, Davis-Keen, & Eccles, 2005; Mau & Bikos, 2000; Patton & Creed, 2007; Perry, Przybysz, & Al-Sheikh, 2009; Ryan, 2000). Further, these academic aspirations and expectations are particularly prone to changes over the course of adolescence (Beal & Crockett, 2010; Cooper, 2009; Eccles, Barber, Stone, & Hunt, 2003; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002). Cognitions about the future (e.g. school completion) take place during adolescence and become increasingly refined, more realistic, based on interests, perceived abilities, individual characteristics, and available opportunities (Crockett & Bingham, 2000; Eccles
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
et al., 2003; Erikson, 1968; Nurmi, 2004). Adolescents' thoughts about their future might be important because they could influence outcomes such as choices, decisions, and activities, that may affect subsequent accomplishments and achievements (Beal & Crockett, 2010; Little, 2007; Nurmi, 2004). Moreover, both cognitive and non-cognitive factors contribute to the prediction of scholastic achievement with greater or smaller intensity depending on the considered educational stage (e.g., O'Conner & Paunonen, 2007). Thus, for instance, cognitive ability is usually less related with scholastic achievement at higher educational levels (Almeida, Guisande, Primi, & Lemos, 2008; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005; Laidra et al., 2007; Lemos et al., 2010), which can be explained by a number of reasons. The first is related to the restriction of ability range (Almeida et al., 2008; Boekaerts, 1995) meaning that students in higher levels of schooling are more alike regarding their general cognitive ability. A plausible argument to explain this homogeneity is that students with lower cognitive abilities choose alternative educational curricula usually not included in the regular samples of high school students, or contribute to early dropout. Another argument follows the “law of diminishing returns”. Originally proposed by Charles Spearman (1927), who reported that the average correlation between 12 cognitive ability tests was .466 in 78 normal children, and .782 in 22 “defective” children, this law (Spearman's Law of Diminishing Returns, SLDR) predicts that the g factor will account for a smaller proportion of individual differences in cognitive test scores at higher levels. The decreasing prediction of g at high educational levels may be due to smaller correlations among abilities in the more intelligent. These results were replicated elsewhere in a variety of children and adult samples (Deary & Pagliari, 1991; Detterman & Daniel, 1989; Tucker-Drob, 2009, but see Abad, Colom, Juan-Espinosa, & García, 2003). Secondly, the decreasing correlation between cognitive ability and scholastic achievement at higher educational levels can also be explained by the Gf–Gc investment theory (Cattell, 1971). This theory suggests a diminishing relevance of fluid intelligence (Gf) due to the emergence and development of crystallized intelligence (Gc), more involved with consolidated knowledge obtained by education, experience and interests throughout adolescence. The elementary school learning inputs can be understood as basic acquisitions, less centered in content than in form, strongly associated with the exercise of basic processes in perception, memory, and reasoning, and easily confounded with fluid intelligence (Gf). In learning inputs and scholastic achievement starting in adolescence, considering a curriculum which grows exponentially both in amplitude and complexity, knowledge and experience are requested to a greater extent — crystallized intelligence (Gc), or specific skills are predominant. Understanding Gf more like “inductive reasoning” and Gc as “acculturation knowledge” (Horn & Noll, 1997) fits this explanation that illustrates the progressive importance of knowledge, contents and domains of cognitive problems (Ackerman, 1996; Beauducel, Brocke, & Liepmann, 2001; Cattell, 1987; Gustafsson, 1984; Guttman & Levy, 1991; Schweizer & Koch, 2001). A third explanation is concerned with the increased contribution of further psychological factors associated with the learning process across school levels, such as educational aspirations and expectations, students' beliefs, motivation, study habits, students' approaches to learning, or vocational choices (Chamorro-Premuzic & Arteche, 2008; Eccles et al., 2003; Entwistle, Tait, & McCune, 2000; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Freiberger et al., 2012; O'Conner & Paunonen, 2007; Rosander & Bäckström, 2012; Steinmayr & Spinath, 2009). It is noteworthy that previous research has often neglected the analysis of the relation between cognitive ability and academic achievement taking into account variables such as past academic failure and future academic aspirations. For filling this gap, the present study comprehensively investigates the relationships among cognitive ability, past academic failure, future academic aspirations, and present scholastic achievement analyzing representative samples of students. To study the interplay of cognitive ability, past academic failure, future academic
149
aspirations and scholastic achievement, two main models were tested: one that posits the exclusive relevance or direct effect of g, the named “only-g” model, and another model holding the relevance of further cognitive and other variables besides g, the named “non-only-g” model. Therefore, the present study tests if these two models are both suitable in predicting scholastic achievement on Language and Math, and, in particular, if both models have good fit indexes at the beginning and at the end of adolescence (young and old adolescents' samples, respectively). The potential contribution of specific cognitive skills is also addressed because, as noted above, they may differentially contribute to the prediction of different academic subjects (Bull & Johnston, 1997; Bull, Johnston, & Roy, 1999; Campos, Almeida, Ferreira, Martinez, & Ramalho, 2013; Geary, Hamson, & Hoard, 2000; Henry & MacLean, 2003; Kyttälä & Lehto, 2008; Rothstein, Paunonen, Rush, & King, 1994). 2. Method 2.1. Participants Two independent samples comprising 2796 students were considered. The first sample included 1695 students from the third cycle of elementary school (young adolescents; mean age = 13.5, SD = .97, range from 12 to 15 years) and the second sample comprised 1101 students from secondary school (old adolescents; mean age = 16.8, SD = .82, range from 16 to 19 years). All participants were involved in a larger study for the standardization of the Reasoning Test Battery (RTB; Almeida & Lemos, 2007) and answered some questions about past academic failure, future academic aspirations, and academic achievement. The samples were obtained randomly and state schools were selected considering previous stratification by regions in the country, school grade and gender within the class group at the school level. According to the annual school census of the Department of Assessment and Foresight and Planning – Ministry of Education – samples gather 6% of the Portuguese student population in the considered school levels. The school system in Portugal considers three cycles in elementary school and one cycle in secondary school. The present study takes students from the 3rd cycle of elementary school, equivalent to junior high school in other countries (7th–9th grades), and secondary school (10th–12th grades), when students choose from among several curricular options in order to follow different graduation areas in higher education or professional specialization. The first school level corresponds to the first sample mentioned above, whereas the second level matches the second sample. 2.2. Measures Intelligence was assessed through the Reasoning Tests Battery (RTB). The young adolescents performed the version designed for the first level (3rd cycle of elementary school) and the old adolescents performed the version designed for the second level (senior high school battery). In both cases, the RTB consists of five reasoning time-limited subtests: abstract reasoning (AR, 25 figural analogies and 5 min of administration time), numerical reasoning (NR, 20 numerical series and 10 min of administration time), verbal reasoning (VR, 25 verbal analogies and 4 min of administration time), mechanical reasoning (MR, 25 mechanical problem-solving items and 8 min of administration time), and spatial reasoning (SR, 20 spatial orientation and cube rotation series and 9 min of administration time). Fig. 1 shows examples of items from these subtests. Reliability indices were computed by test–retest and internal consistency methods. Obtained indices ranged from .63 (mechanical reasoning subtest) to .84 (numerical reasoning subtest). Factor analysis computed from different samples confirmed a single factor explaining between 50 and 60% of the variance (Almeida & Lemos, 2007). Previous confirmatory factor analysis confirms this general factor of intelligence (g) that predicts the five measures comprised in the battery in both
150
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
Example of an Abstract Reasoning item
Example of a Numerical Reasoning item
Example of a Verbal Reasoning item
A. Marketer
4. Doctor is to patient as seller is to... B. Receipt C. Merchandise D. Customer E. Tradesman
Example of a Mechanical Reasoning item
Which pair of strings (A, B, C, D) actually prevents the advertising plaque from sagging? A. Strings 1 and 2 B. Strings 3 and 2 C. Strings 1 and 4 D. Strings 2 and 4 Example of a Spatial Reasoning item
Fig. 1. Example items from the administered intelligence battery.
samples (Lemos, Almeida, & Colom, 2011). A general score was computed summing the five reasoning standardized tests and this score was converted to the IQ scale (Mean = 100; SD = 15) for clarity purposes (see Colom et al., 2010). Past academic failure was measured by an item asking students to indicate whether they had ever failed a school year in their academic journey (1 = past failure, 0 = no failure). Future academic aspirations were measured by an item asking students to indicate how far in school they want to go: (1) until 9th grade or final of basic education (only applicable for young adolescents), (2) until 12th grade or final of secondary education, (3) to do a professional course, or (4) to do an academic higher education course. Present academic achievement was assessed by students' grades on Language (Portuguese) and Math.
2.3. Procedures A team of psychologists was trained for the administration of the intelligence battery by means of an 8 hour lasting training course. Before administration itself, strictly adhering to conditions specified in the battery's manual, participants were acquainted with the study's main aims as well as ethical concerns such as voluntary collaboration and data confidentiality. Then participants completed some brief questions about their past and present academic achievement and also about their future academic aspirations. Administration of the RTB and brief academic achievement questionnaire was completed by participants in classes with no more than 25 students during regular teaching hours, with the agreement of teachers. Schools' Executive Councils,
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
and when necessary (students less than 16 years old) students' carers provided written informed consent. 2.4. Statistical analyses Several structural equation models were tested separately for the young and old adolescents, since some of the measures of interest were not exactly the same for both groups. First, the model depicted in Fig. 2 (excluding dotted arrows) was applied (“only-g” model). In this model, past academic failure and future academic aspirations predict present academic achievement in Math and Language. Furthermore, a general factor of intelligence (g) defined by the five cognitive measures [abstract reasoning (AR), numerical reasoning (NR), verbal reasoning (VR), mechanical reasoning (MR), and spatial reasoning (SR)] predicts directly past academic failure, future academic aspirations and present academic achievement. In the “only-g” model the specificity of each psychometric test does not predict Language and Math. In the “non-only-g” model, two theoretical plausible relationships are added (VR → Language and NR → Math). Model fit was assessed using the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI). Values close to .95 for CFI and below .06 for RMSEA suggest a good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The Akaike Information Criteria (AIC; Akaike, 1987) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Raftery, 1995) were also used for comparing models. AIC and BIC are sensitive to model parsimony. When comparing models, the smaller these indices the better the fit. All models were fitted using maximum likelihood with Mplus (Muthen & Muthen, 2012). Percentile-based bias-corrected bootstrap Confidence Intervals were obtained to test indirect effects (Hayes, 2009; Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010). Equality of standardized regression weights was tested by creating new parameters with the NEW option in Mplus (Cheung, 2009). 3. Results The descriptive statistics along with the correlation matrix are shown in Table 1. Note that past academic failure was .20 for both young and old adolescents indicating that 80% of students tend to succeed in both junior high school and secondary school. The mean scores for future academic aspirations (young adolescents, 3.5 and old adolescents, 3.8) showed
Fig. 2. The “only-g” model (excluding dashed lines) and the “non-only-g model” (including all paths). Residual variances for predicted variables are not represented.
151
that students from junior high school and secondary school want to follow a post-secondary professional course or an academic higher education course. Regarding the correlations among the study variables, it can be seen that numerical reasoning is correlated with Math (young adolescents r = .37; old adolescents r = .31) and verbal reasoning is correlated with Language (young adolescents r = .37; old adolescents r = .33). Correlations between cognitive ability and academic achievement slightly decrease for the old adolescents (e.g., IQ score correlations with Math were .40 and .33 for the young and the old adolescents, respectively). Correlations between past academic failure and future academic aspirations decrease for the old adolescents (young adolescents, −.40 and old adolescents, −.28). Math and Language show a high correlation for both samples (around .60). In the next step, structural equation models were tested. Fit indices are reported in Table 2. Afterwards, we computed multiple regression models (Language and Math on g, past failure and future aspirations) for the young and old adolescents. For both the groups, this model showed a good fit (CFI N .95; RMSEA around 0.06). For the young adolescents, standardized weights were significant for Language (g: .28; future aspirations: .19; past failure: −.15) and Math (g: .36; future aspirations: .18; past failure: − .13) explaining 22% of Language performance and 27% of Math performance. Correlations among predictors were significant (g correlated .32 and −.33 with future aspirations and past failure, respectively; future aspirations and past failure correlated − .40). For the old adolescents, standardized weights were significant on Language (g: .22; future aspirations: .18; past failure: − .22) and Math (g: .31; future aspirations: .14; past failure: −.12) explaining 19% of Language performance and 17% of Math performance. Correlations among predictors were significant (g correlated .23 and −.26 with future aspirations and past failure, respectively; future aspirations and past failure correlated −.28). With respect to the mediation models (only-g and non-only-g) these were the main results for both groups. Regarding the young adolescents, both mediation models showed a good fit (CFI N .95; RMSEA around .06), but the “only-g” model showed a worse fit than the “non-only-g” model. The final non-only-g model is depicted in Fig. 3a. According to the bootstrap confidence intervals, all the tested indirect (mediation) effects were significant (p b 0.01) and all the standardized factor loadings and regression weights were significant (p b .01). Standardized regression weights of past failure (B = − .33) and future aspirations (B = .32) on g were equal in size (p N .05). Regression weights of Language (B = .19, −.14, .11) and Math (B = .18, −.14, .26) were equal on aspirations (p N .05), on past fail (p N 0.5) but not on g (p b .01). All the predictors (g, VR, NR, past failure, and aspirations) explained 22% of Language and 26% of Math. Excluding past failure and future aspirations reduced the percentage of explained variance to 15%, for Language, and 20%, for Math. Of the total effects of g on Language, 31% were direct, 39% were mediated by VR, and the remaining mediated by aspirations (17%) and past failure (13%). Of the total effects of g on Math, 59% were direct, 18% were mediated by NR, and the remaining mediated by aspirations (13%) or past failure (10%). For the old adolescents, both models showed a good fit, but the “non-only g” model fitted better. The final non-only g model is depicted in Fig. 3b. All the tested indirect (mediation) effects were significant with p b 0.01 or p b .05 (g → NR → Math) and all the standardized factor loadings and regression weights were significant with p b .01 or p b .05 (NR → Math). Standardized regression weights of past failure (B = − .26) and future aspirations (B = .23) on g were equal in size (p N .05). Regression weights of Language (B = .18, .14, − .22) and Math (B = .15, .23, −.12) were equal on aspirations (p N .05) and g (p N .05) but not on past failure (p b .01). All the predictors (g, VR, NR, past failure, and aspirations) explained 20% of Language and 17% of Math. Excluding past failure and future aspirations reduced the percentage of explained variance to 11% for Language, and to 13% for Math. Of the total effects of g on Language, 46% were direct, 22% were
152
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficients of both samples. AR
NR
VR
MR
SR
Fail
Aspir
Lang
Math
IQ
– .46 .47 .37 .43 −.22 .19 .21 .27 .75
.38 – .45 .38 .47 −.22 .24 .26 .37 .75
.29 .33 – .40 .40 −.30 .28 .37 .32 .74
.32 .40 .31 – .36 −.07 .08 .15 .19 .69
.49 .50 .33 .44 – −.21 .20 .24 .30 .73
−.16 −.19 −.17 −.11 −.19 – −.40 −.31 −.32 −.28
.17 .12 .16 .10 .17 −.28 – .34 .35 .27
.13 .25 .33 .15 .19 −.33 .29 − .58 .34
.19 .31 .32 .14 .23 −.24 .24 .60 – .40
.70 .73 .64 .70 .78 −.23 .21 .30 .33 –
Young adolescents (N = 1695) Mean 12.7 SD 3.2
8.6 3.7
14 3.7
9.3 3.2
9.6 4
0.2 0.4
3.5 0.8
3.1 0.7
3.1 0.9
100 15
Old adolescents (N = 1101) Mean 12.1 SD 2.8
9.6 3.6
15.6 3.3
9.7 3.5
10.4 3.5
0.2 0.4
3.8 0.5
12.5 2.8
12 3.5
100 15
AR NR VR MR SR Failure Aspirations Language Math IQ
Note. Correlations below the diagonal are for young adolescents. Correlations above the diagonal are for old adolescents.
mediated by VR, 13% were mediated by future aspirations and 19% were mediated by past failure. Of the total effects of g on Math, 65% were direct, 17% were mediated by NR, 9% were mediated by future aspirations and 9% were mediated by past failure. Finally, because the administered tests differed across age groups, measurement invariance tests were not applicable. Nevertheless, the equality of within-group standardized loadings and regression weights across age groups was tested using a multi-group model. The model is depicted in Fig. 3c. Results showed that standardized regression weights were remarkably similar for both groups, with minor exceptions: (a) aspirations on g (.31 vs .24; p b .05), (b) Language on past failure (−.14 vs − .23, p b .05), (c) AR on g (.66 vs .50; p b .01), (d) VR on g (.67 vs .55; p b .01) and (e) SR on g (.64 vs .75; p b .01). In summary, the reported analyses support the perspective that previous academic failure and future academic aspirations contribute to the prediction of scholastic achievement. The variance explained considering all the significant predictors was: (a) for the young adolescents 26% (Math) and 22% (Language) and (b) for the old adolescents 17% (Math) and 20% (Language). The percentage of explained variance was reduced to 4–9%, when excluding past failures and future aspirations. 4. Discussion Here we have shown that cognitive ability differences, past academic failure and future academic aspirations are related with present academic achievement. These simultaneous relationships are generally invariant across the age groups considered. Beyond this general conclusion, there are several noteworthy points that can be derived from the reported results. First, general cognitive ability is a direct predictor of scholastic achievement for the young and old adolescents. This result can be
Table 2 Fit indices for the young and old adolescents. X2
df
RMSEA
CFI
SRMR
AIC
BIC
Young adolescents “Only-g”a 173.1 “Non-only g” 103.3
21 19
0.065 0.051
0.961 0.979
0.033 0.028
55875.6 55809.8
56055.0 56000.0
Old adolescents “Only-g”a 116.0 “Non-only g” 86.6
21 19
0.064 0.057
0.956 0.969
0.039 0.033
40862.0 40836.6
41027.1 41011.7
Note. a Fit equivalent to the multiple regression model.
framed within R. B. Cattell's investment theory (Cattell, 1987). This theory suggests that abstract or fluid reasoning evolves to knowledgebased problem solving (crystallized ability) throughout the developmental process. As adolescents get older, specific abilities grounded on education, experience, and interests may overcome fluid intelligence. An increased specialization/differentiation of cognitive abilities is expected as individuals aged and pass through cultural exposure and the educational curricula (Schweizer & Koch, 2001). This is not particularly consistent with the findings observed here because a greater relevance of verbal and numerical reasoning for the old adolescents is expected with respect to their Language and Math academic achievement. Secondly, past academic failure and future academic aspirations are predicted by general cognitive ability (g) with values ranging from .24 (old adolescents) to .31 (young adolescents). Therefore, the higher the general ability (a) the greater the future academic aspirations and (b) the lower the likelihood of having experienced past academic failure. Table 1 showed direct correlations between future academic aspirations and present scholastic achievement (between .24 and .35) as well as between the latter and past academic failure (between − .24 and −.33). These values are reduced when general cognitive ability is considered as a covariate (see Fig. 3a, b, and c) but they are still statistically significant. Thus, the predictive validity of past academic failure and future academic aspirations should not be minimized. Excluding these variables from the prediction model reduced several points the percentage of explained variance (Language and Math) for the young and old groups. Third, the relationships among many of the variables of interest are generally invariant across age (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005; Eccles et al., 2003; Laidra et al., 2007; O'Conner & Paunonen, 2007; Rosander & Bäckström, 2012; Steinmayr & Spinath, 2009): (a) general cognitive ability predicts past academic failure with a value of − .30, (b) general cognitive ability predicts future academic aspirations with values of .31 and .24 for the young and old adolescents respectively, and (c) general cognitive ability is directly related with academic achievement in the young and old adolescents (.14 for Language and .25 for Math), (d) verbal reasoning and numerical reasoning are also predictors of Language (.17) and Math (.10) respectively, and (e) abstract reasoning, spatial reasoning, and mechanical reasoning are unrelated with present academic achievement, as measured by Language and Math, mainly because their relevant variance is captured by g for the young and old adolescents. In summary, here we have shown that the analysis of representative samples of students highlights the multivariate nature of academic achievement. Individual differences in the learning outcomes involved in Language and Math academic subjects are predicted by cognitive
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
153
Fig. 3. (a) Model for the young adolescents (N = 1695), (b) model for the old adolescents (N = 1101), and (c) multi-group model. For non-invariant parameters, values between parentheses are for the old adolescents.
abilities, past academic failure, and future academic aspirations. Students with higher cognitive scores, less academic previous failures and greater future academic aspirations do better in school, as measured by their performance in Language and Math. Improving the latter is openly relevant for society, as persistently claimed by international research such as PISA. It is true that academic achievement is multivariate in nature, but the relevant variables must be hierarchically organized for a proper understanding of the involved factors. Previous academic failure and future academic aspirations considered in the present study are relevant, but nonetheless they are less important than cognitive abilities. Thus, improving students' academic performance must begin with the close analysis of how their individual differences in these
cognitive abilities make a substantial contribution. Indeed, it is reasonable to suspect that past academic failure and future academic aspirations are influenced by the students' cognitive abilities. The best way for promoting these two factors might require finding ways for overcoming the students' cognitive limitations by the manipulation of their instructional environment (Snow, 1989). References Abad, F. J., Colom, R., Juan-Espinosa, M., & García, L. F. (2003). Intelligence differentiation in adult samples. Intelligence, 31, 157–166. Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 227–257.
154
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52(3), 317–332. Almeida, L. S., Guisande, M.A., Primi, R., & Lemos, G. C. (2008). Contribuciones del factor general y de los factores especificos en la relación entre inteligencia y rendimiento escolar. European Journal of Education and Psychology, 1, 5–16. Almeida, L. S., & Lemos, G. C. (2007). Bateria de Provas de Raciocínio: Manual. Braga: Universidade do Minho. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Beal, S. J., & Crockett, L. J. (2010). Adolescents' occupational and educational aspirations and expectations: Links to high school activities and adult educational attainment. Developmental Psychology, 46, 258–265. Beauducel, A., Brocke, B., & Liepmann, D. (2001). Perspectives on fluid and crystallized intelligence: Facets for verbal, numerical, and figural intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 977–994. Black, S. (2002). When students push past peer influence. The Education Digest, 68, 31–36. Boekaerts, M. (1995). The interface between intelligence and personality as determinants of classroom learning. In D. H. Saklofske, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 161–183). New York: Plenum. Bratko, D., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Saks, Z. (2006). Personality and school performance: Incremental validity of self- and peer-ratings over intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 131–142. Bull, R., & Johnston, R. S. (1997). Children's arithmetical difficulties: Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 1–24. Bull, R., Johnston, R. S., & Roy, J. A. (1999). Exploring the roles of the visual-spatial sketch pad and central executive in children's arithmetical skills: Views from cognition and developmental neuropsychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 15(3), 421–442. Campos, I. S., Almeida, L. S., Ferreira, A. I., Martinez, L. F., & Ramalho, G. (2013). Cognitive processes and math performance: A study with children at third grade of basic education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28(2), 421–436. Cattell, R. B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality. Harmondsworth: Penguin. Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth and action. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers BV. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Arteche, A. (2008). Intellectual competence and academic performance. Intelligence, 36, 564–573. Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and intellectual competence. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Chen, Z. -Y., & Kaplan, H. B. (2003). School failure in early adolescence and status attainment in middle adulthood: A longitudinal study. Sociology of Education, 76, 110–127. Cheng, S., & Starks, B. (2002). Racial differences in the effects of significant others on students' educational expectations. Sociology of Education, 75(4), 306–327. Cheung, M. W. L. (2009). Constructing approximate confidence intervals for parameters with structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 16(2), 267–294. Colom, R., & Flores-Mendoza, C. E. (2007). Intelligence predicts scholastic achievement irrespective of SES factors: Evidence from Brazil. Intelligence, 35, 243–251. Colom, R., Quiroga, Mª. A., Shih, P. C., Martínez, K., Burgaleta, M., Martínez-Molina, A., et al. (2010). Improvement in working memory is not related to increased intelligence scores. Intelligence, 38, 497–505. Conard, M.A. (2006). Aptitude is not enough: How personality and behaviour predict academic performance. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 339–346. Cooper, M.A. (2009). Dreams deferred? The relationship between early and later postsecondary educational aspirations among racial/ethnic groups. Educational Policy, 23, 615–650. Crockett, L. J., & Bingham, C. R. (2000). Anticipating adulthood: Expected timing of work and family transitions among rural youth. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10, 151–172. Danziger, N., & Eden, Y. (2007). Gender-related differences in the career aspirations and career-style preferences of accounting students. Career Development International, 12(2), 129–149. Deary, I. J., & Pagliari, C. (1991). The strength of g at different levels of ability: Have Detterman and Daniel rediscovered Spearman's “law of diminishing returns”? Intelligence, 15, 247–250. Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35, 13–21. Detterman, D. K., & Daniel, M. H. (1989). Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low-IQ groups. Intelligence, 13, 349–359. Eccles, J. S., Barber, B.L., Stone, M., & Hunt, J. (2003). Extracurricular activities and adolescent development. Journal of Social Issues, 59, 865–889. Entwistle, N. J., Tait, H., & McCune, V. (2000). Patterns of response to an approaches to studying inventory across contrasting groups and contexts. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 15, 33–48. Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton. Farsides, T., & Woodfield, R. (2003). Individual differences and undergraduate academic success: The roles of personality, intelligence, and application. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1225–1243. Feliciano, C., & Rumbaut, R. G. (2005). Gendered paths: Educational and occupational expectations and outcomes among adult children of immigrants. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 28, 1087–1118. Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children's competence and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth trajectories in two male-sex-typed domains. Developmental Psychology, 38, 519–533. Freiberger, V., Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2012). Competence beliefs and perceived ability evaluations: How do they contribute to intrinsic motivation and achievement? Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 518–522.
Furnham, A., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2004). Personality and intelligence as predictors of statistics examination grades. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1013–1022. Furnham, A., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & McDougall, F. (2003). Personality, cognitive ability, and beliefs about intelligence as predictors of academic performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 14, 47–64. Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, M. K. (2000). Numerical and arithmetical cognition: A longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 236–263. Gilles, P. Y., & Bailleux, C. (2001). Personality traits and abilities as predictors of academic achievement. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 16, 3–15. Goldstein, S. E., Davis-Keen, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Parents, peers, and problem behavior: A longitudinal investigation of the impact of relationship perceptions and characteristics on the development of adolescent problem behavior. Developmental Psychology, 41, 401–413. Gustafsson, J. E. (1984). A unifying model for the structure of intellectual abilities. Intelligence, 8, 179–203. Guttman, L., & Levy, S. (1991). Two structural laws for intelligence tests. Intelligence, 15, 79–103. Harris, D. (1940). Factors affecting college grades: A review of the literature, 1930–1937. Psychological Bulletin, 37, 125–166. Haveman, R., & Smeeding, T. (2006). The role of higher education in social mobility. Future of Children, 16, 125–150. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420. Heaven, P. C. L., & Ciarrochi, J. (2012). When IQ is not everything: Intelligence, personality and academic performance at school. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 518–522. Henry, L. A., & MacLean, M. (2003). Relationships between working memory, expressive vocabulary and arithmetical reasoning in children with and without intellectual disabilities. Educational and Child Psychology, 20, 51–64. Horn, J. L., & Noll, J. (1997). Human cognitive capabilities: Gf–Gc theory. In D. P. Flanagan, J. L. Genshaft & P. L. Harrison (Orgs.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: theories, tests, and issues (pp. 53–91). New York: Guilford. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. Jensen, A.R. (1998). The g factor: The Science of mental ability. Westport, C. T.: Praeger. Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor and the design of education. In R. J. Sternberg, & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Intelligence, instruction and assessment (pp. 111–131). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. Kane, H. D., & Brand, C. R. (2006). The variable importance of general intelligence (g) in the cognitive ability of children and adolescents. Educational Psychology, 26(6), 751–767. Kao, G., & Thompson, J. S. (2003). Racial and ethnic stratification in educational achievement and attainment. Annual Review of Sociology, 29, 417–442. Kappe, F. R., & van der Flier, H. (2012). Predicting academic success in higher education: What's more important than being smart? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 27, 605–619. Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 148–161. Kyttälä, M., & Lehto, J. E. (2008). Some factors underlying mathematical performance: The role of visuospatial working memory and non-verbal intelligence. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 23(1), 77–94. Laidra, K., Pillmann, H., & Allik, J. (2007). Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 441–451. Lee, J. O., Hill, K. G., & Hawkins, J.D. (2012). The role of educational aspirations and expectations in the discontinuity of intergenerational low-income status. Social Work Research, 36(2), 141–151. Lemos, G. C., Almeida, L. S., & Colom, R. (2011). Intelligence of adolescents is related to their parents' educational level but not to family income. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(7), 1062–1067. Lemos, G. C., Almeida, L. S., Guisande, M.A., Barca, A., Primi, R., Martinho, G., et al. (2010). Inteligência e rendimento escolar: Contingências de um relacionamento menos óbvio no final da adolescência. Revista Galego-Portuguesa de Psicoloxía e Educación, 18(1), 163–175. Little, B. R. (2007). Prompt and circumstance: The generative contexts of personal projects analysis. In B. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, & S. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, actions, and human flourishing (pp. 3–49). Mahwah, N. J.: Erlbaum. MacLeod, J. (1995). Ain't no makin' it: Aspirations and attainment in a low-income neighborhood (2nd ed.). Boulder, C. O.: Westview Press. Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (2012). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. Mau, W. -C., & Bikos, L. H. (2000). Educational and vocational aspirations of minority and female students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Development, 78, 186–194. Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., et al. (1996). Intelligence, knowns, and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101. Noftle, E., & Robins, R. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big Five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 116–130. Nurmi, J. -E. (2004). Socialization and self-development: Channeling, selection, adjustment, and reflection. In R. Lerner, & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (pp. 85–124) (2nd ed.). Hoboken, N. J.: Wiley. O'Conner, M. C., & Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big Five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 971–990.
G.C. Lemos et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 32 (2014) 148–155 Patton, W., & Creed, P. (2007). The relationship between career variables and career aspirations and expectations for Australian high school adolescents. Journal of Career Development, 34, 127–148. Perry, J. C., Przybysz, J., & Al-Sheikh, M. (2009). Reconsidering the “aspiration-expectation gap” and assumed gender differences among urban youth. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 74, 349–354. Phillipson, S., & Phillipson, S. N. (2012). Children's cognitive ability and their academic achievement: The mediation effects of parental expectations. Asian Pacific Education Review, 13, 495–508. Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 2, 322–338. Porter, K. (2002). The value of a college degree. ERIC digest. Washington, D. C.: Eric Clearinghouse on Higher Education. Primi, R., Ferrão, M. E., & Almeida, L. S. (2010). Fluid intelligence as a predictor of learning: A longitudinal multilevel approach applied to math. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 446–451. Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–163. Rosander, P., & Bäckström, M. (2012). The unique contribution of learning approaches to academic performance, after controlling for IQ and personality: Are there gender differences? Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 820–826. Rosander, P., Bäckström, M., & Stenberg, G. (2011). Personality traits and general intelligence as predictors of academic performance: A structural equation modeling approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 590–596. Rothon, C., Arephin, M., Klineberg, E., Cattell, V., & Stansfeld, S. (2011). Structural and socio-psychological influences on adolescents' educational aspirations and subsequent academic achievement. Social Psychology of Education, 14(2), 209–231.
155
Rothstein, M., Paunonen, S., Rush, J., & King, G. (1994). Personality and cognitive ability predictors of performance in graduate business school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 516–530. Ryan, A.M. (2000). Peer groups as a context for the socialization of adolescents' motivation, engagement, and achievement in school. Educational Psychologist, 35, 101–111. Schweizer, K., & Koch, W. (2001). A revision of Cattell's investment theory: Cognitive perspectives influencing learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 13(1), 57–82. Snow, R. E. (1989). Aptitude–treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in learning. In P. L. Ackerman, R. J. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Advances in theory and research. New York: Freeman. Spearman, C. E. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan. Steinmayr, R., & Spinath, B. (2009). The importance of motivation as a predictor of school achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(1), 80–90. Taub, G. E., Keith, T. Z., Floyd, R. G., & Mcgrew, K. S. (2008). Effects of general and broad cognitive abilities on mathematics achievement. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 187–198. Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2009). Differentiation of cognitive abilities across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1097–1118. Webb, E. (1915). Character and intelligence: An attempt at an exact study of character. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wilson, K. (2001). The determinants of educational attainment: Modeling and estimating the human capital model and education production functions. Southern Economic Journal, 67, 518–551. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.