Refining the analysis of a whole genome linkage disequilibrium association map: the United Kingdom results

Refining the analysis of a whole genome linkage disequilibrium association map: the United Kingdom results

Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53 – 59 www.elsevier.com/locate/jneuroim Refining the analysis of a whole genome linkage disequilibrium associa...

116KB Sizes 0 Downloads 14 Views

Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53 – 59 www.elsevier.com/locate/jneuroim

Refining the analysis of a whole genome linkage disequilibrium association map: the United Kingdom results Tai Wai Yeo a, Richard Roxburgh a, Mel Maranian a, Sara Singlehurst a, Julia Gray a, Anke Hensiek a, Efrosini Setakis b, Alastair Compston a, Stephen Sawcer a,* b

a University of Cambridge Neurology Unit, Addenbrook’s Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge, CB2 2QQ, UK MRC Biostatistics Unit, Institute of Public Health, University Forvie site, Robinson Way, Cambridge, CB10 1SB, UK

Abstract Individual genotyping of the 10 most promising markers identified in our previously reported screen for linkage disequilibrium (LD) in multiple sclerosis identified a number of effects which confound the analysis and are of general importance in the interpretation of results obtained using microsatellite markers typed in pooled DNA. In order to identify and characterise these effects, we individually genotyped 529 promising markers in 16 trio families. We then devised adapting factors, which were designed to correct for these confounders. This more extensive analysis of the previously published UK data set and the repeat analyses incorporating these adaptations led to the identification of two novel markers that may be associated with multiple sclerosis in this population, providing a close correlation between the results of pooled analysis and individual typing. D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Genome screen; Linkage disequilibrium; Multiple sclerosis; Microsatellites; DNA pooling

1. Introduction In 1997, Barcellos et al. (1997) proposed an efficient method for screening for linkage disequilibrium (LD) in complex traits based on typing a dense map of microsatellite markers in pooled DNA. We have previously used an adapted version of this method to perform two systematic screens for LD in multiple sclerosis. Preliminary results were reported in 2002 (Sawcer et al., 2002). In this screen, allele image patterns (AIPs) generated from separately pooled cases and controls were normalised according to the number of alleles in the corresponding pools. Resulting allele counts were then compared statistically using a chi squared test (alleles with a frequency of less than 5% were grouped together). The first screen employed unrelated cases (n = 216) and controls (n = 219), while the second was based on 745 trio families (an affected individual and their parents) and used the un-transmitted parental alleles as controls, resulting in an analysis equivalent to the AFBAC method proposed by Thomson (1995).

Because pooling introduces additional sources of error, above and beyond sampling variance, it is impossible to establish absolute p-values for the calculated chi squared statistics. However, by using the observed distribution of these statistics, we established significance empirically and thereby ranked markers in terms of their apparent evidence for association—as reflected in the observed difference in case-control AIPs (Setakis, 2003). In order to reduce the effect of additional sources of variance on the final ranking of markers, extra-AIPs were generated for those markers (n = 520) showing the most extreme results. This approach was adopted generically by the GAMES collaborative and used to screen a variety of additional populations. Results from the majority of these screens are reported in this issue. This article describes our efforts to refine results of the previously reported UK screen.

2. Materials and methods 2.1. Samples and markers

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-1223-217091; fax: +44-1223336941. E-mail address: [email protected] (S. Sawcer). 0165-5728/$ - see front matter D 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroim.2003.08.011

The patients and controls employed in this study are as previously described (Sawcer et al., 2002). All are Cauca-

54

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59

sians of British descent and all gave informed consent for genetic analysis. Each patient satisfied Poser criteria for the diagnosis of multiple sclerosis (Poser et al., 1983). The number and specificity of markers are as previously described (Sawcer et al., 2002). Full details are available from our web site (http://www-gene.cimr.cam.ac.uk/MSgenetics/ GAMES). 2.2. Genotyping DNA extraction and pooling are as previously described (Sawcer et al., 2002). Briefly, PCR amplification was performed using TrueAllele PCR premix (Applied Biosystems) according to the manufacturers recommended methods on 9700 thermal cyclers (Applied Biosystems). Electrophoresis was performed on a 3700 DNA analyser (Applied Biosystems). Both pooled and individual genotyping were performed according to the same conditions, although limited multiplexing was employed for individual genotyping of the best markers. Individual genotyping of the 16 trio families was performed without multiplexing. Analysis of electropherograms was performed using the GENESCAN and GENOTYPER software packages (Applied Biosystems). Where additional AIPs were required a total of four PCR replicates was generated from case-control pools, after which each PCR product was electrophoresed twice in order to generate up to eight new AIP replicates from each pool. Double this number of PCR replicates was generated from the trio family pools, each being electrophoresed twice; the greater number of individuals making up the trio family pools demands more replicate AIPs (Barratt et al., 2002).

3. Results 3.1. Analysis by repeat class Inspection of our previously published analysis (Sawcer et al., 2002) reveals a statistically significant under-representation of dinucleotide markers amongst those at the extreme end of the ranking (see Table 1). This distortion in the expected proportions suggests that markers behave differently in a pooling experiment according to repeat class. It therefore follows that estimating the empirical significance from a distribution including all classes of repeats is likely to favour those classes of repeats showing the greatest variance. This problem is further compounded by the fact that the proportion of dinucleotides varies according to the number of degrees of freedom; that is, the number of common alleles identified for each microsatellite. Amongst markers with low degrees of freedom the proportion of dinucleotides was 40– 50%, compared with 80– 90% amongst those showing high degrees of freedom. The dinucleotides with low degrees of freedom are thus particularly under represented in the list of extreme results identified in our previous analysis. Separate analysis of the data from each class of repeat confirms the marked difference in error structure between the repeat classes and substantially changes the overall marker ranking. A total of 104 markers not previously included amongst those for which we have already generated refining AIPs emerged as interesting in this repeat class specific analysis. Additional AIPs were therefore generated for these markers and added to the existing UK data. 3.2. Individual typing of markers emerging from the original UK screens

2.3. Statistical analysis The adapting factors were incorporated into the statistical analysis of AIP data, as follows. First, signal from peaks determined to be erroneous were excluded from the raw AIP data. Secondly, when required, correction for length dependent amplification was achieved by multiplying the signal from each retained peak in each AIP by an appropriate factor. For the ith allele in each AIP, this factor=(1 + f*li), where f = the calculated proportional reduction factor for that marker and li = the difference in length (in base pairs) between the ith allele and the shortest allele included in the AIP (allele 1). Finally, these edited data were analysed using the method developed by Setakis (2003) but with each marker normalised according to the ‘‘effective’’ number of alleles in the corresponding pools. This is the actual number reduced by the estimated proportion of alleles not contributing to the AIP. For example, in the analysis of a marker where the AIP captures only 90% of the observed alleles, peak heights would be normalised to 90% of the allele counts in the respective pools.

We individually genotyped the 10 most promising markers identified in our previous report (Sawcer et al., 2002) in all samples used to create the original pools still available: 207 cases (96%), 160 controls (73%) and all 745 trio families. The results are shown in Table 2. In line with the statistical analysis performed in our original study alleles with a frequency of < 5% were combined and global chisquared statistics calculated for each marker in both cohorts. In the trio families, index cases were compared with the untransmitted parental alleles using the AFBAC program Table 1 Number of markers by repeat class

Dinucleotides Trinucleotides Tetranucleotides

Total (6000) N (%)

Most promising (659) N (%)a

Typed novel (7) N (%)

4723 (79) 258 (4) 1019 (17)

463 (70) 45 (7) 151 (23)

1 (14) 3 (43) 3 (43)

a Markers showing empirical p-values of < 5% in either (or both) cohorts (n = 659) in the original UK screen (Sawcer et al., 2002).

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59 Table 2 p-values from individual typing of top UK markers Marker

Case-controls

Trio families

D1S1590 GGAA30B06 D2S2739 D4S416 D6S1615a D6S2444a TNFaa D17S1535 GCT6E11 D19S585

0.81 0.002 0.01 0.25 0.03 6.15E 0.02 0.60 0.56 0.12

0.13 0.47 0.79 0.10 6.12E 6.59E 3.87E 0.39 0.34 0.77

a

05

12 14 09

These three markers are all from the HLA region on 6p21.

(Thomson, 1995). As expected, association with the three HLA makers was confirmed. However, after correction for multiple testing, none of the seven novel markers retain evidence for association. A number of un-anticipated effects were observed, providing an explanation for these reductionist results. Two of the seven novel markers were found to have genotype frequencies, which deviated significantly from Hardy Weinberg equilibrium (D19S585 and GGAA30B06). In the trio family analysis, the presence of null or very poorly amplified alleles could be inferred for these markers, indicating that a proportion of alleles in each pool failed to contribute to the observed AIPs. Since peak heights in an AIP are normalised on the assumption that they reflect all alleles in a pool, allele count differences will be over estimated when a substantial proportion do not contribute to the measured AIPs. Other effects also result in alleles failing to contribute to AIPs. For marker GCT6E11, length dependant amplification resulted in the longest common allele being missed from the AIP. This allele did generate a peak in the electropherogram but so small that it was only measurable in a proportion of replicates and therefore was not included amongst peaks considered to make up the AIP for this marker. Similarly, for marker D2S2739, a total of 16 peaks were included in the AIP although various smaller peaks could be seen in some electropherograms. Individual typing, however, revealed a total of 41 different alleles indicating that some 61% of the possible alleles had not been included in the AIP. As well as missed alleles, we also observed that some of the peaks included in AIPs had no corresponding allele. For marker D1S1590, the majority of individual genotypes included an additional fragment, presumably arising from non-specific amplification. Because of its position, this fragment had mistakenly been labelled as an allele in the AIPs. For marker D4S416, a number of peaks included in the AIP had no corresponding allele being purely stutter bands from other alleles. The erroneous inclusion of these additional peaks as alleles increases the number of degrees of freedom in testing these markers. Since significance is judged against the distribution of markers with the same number of degrees of freedom, this effect distorts the result

55

for that marker and all others in the class. Finally, reinspection of the AIP data for marker D17S1535 revealed a genotyping error which, when corrected, left this marker showing no statistically significant difference in the AIPs from either the case-control or trio family cohorts, in agreement with individual typing results. 3.3. Estimating adapting factors to correct for these phenomena The phenomena described above can be expected variably to affect the remaining 5990 markers employed in our original LD screen. In an effort to estimate the scale of these effects, and thereby appropriately correct for them, we individually genotyped a selection of markers in 16 trio families. This number of families provides a total of 64 independent chromosomes (from 32 unrelated individuals) and thereby a >80% power to observe alleles with a frequency of >2.5% (and at least 50% power to detect alleles with a >1% frequency). Trio families were typed rather than unrelated individuals in order more easily to identify null alleles and contaminating artefacts. Comparison of alleles seen in the individual genotyping with the pattern suggested by the AIP allowed easy identification of peaks erroneously included in the AIP, and provided an estimate of the proportion of alleles not contributing to the measured AIP. Knowledge of this proportion enables an appropriate reduction in the normalisation of AIPs, restricting allele counts to just that faction influencing the AIP. As well as excluding peaks that do not correspond to observed alleles, we also excluded peaks where a corresponding allele was identified but only accounted for less than a quarter of the observed peak height. We reasoned that since such peaks principally reflect stutter artefact from longer alleles, including such data was more harmful than dropping the peak and thereby failing to measure a rare allele. Table 3 Mean and standard deviation (S.D.) for adapting factors estimated from consecutive 16 family subsets of the 745 UK trio families Missed allele proportion

D1S1590 GGAA30B06 D2S2739 D4S416 D6S1615 D6S2444 TNFa D17S1535 GCT6E11 D19S585 a

Relative signal reduction per base pair

Mean

S.D.a

Mean

S.D.

0.082 0.001 0.196 0.001 0.106 0.001 0.027 0 0.416 0.043

0.035 0.003 0.053 0.004 0.044 0.003 0.018 NAb 0.12 0.025

0.051 0.015 0.019 0.039 0.037 0.059 0.028 0.025 0.053 0.04

0.004 0.003 0.003 0.006 0.002 0.004 0.009 0.005 0.017 0.006

As expected, these SD estimates are in close agreement with those predicted for a binomial distribution. b As no missed alleles were seen in any of the replicate data sets for this marker, no deviation was observed and therefore no S.D. estimate can be calculated.

56

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59

Table 4 Marker

Case-controls

Trio families

(a) Markers with final empirical p-values < 10% in both case-control and trio family cohorts. Analysis was performed without correction for length dependant amplification D5S112 0.0281 0.0930 D6S1615a 0.0001 0.0395 D6S2444a 0.0310 0.0777 TNFaa 0.0180 0.0259 (b) Markers with final empirical p-values < 10% amplification D5S112 D6S1615a TNFaa D10S1769 a

in both case-control and trio family cohorts. Analysis was performed with correction for length dependant 0.0255 0.0001 0.0148 0.0664

0.0926 0.0404 0.0307 0.0780

These three markers are from the HLA region on 6p21.

Assuming a linear relationship between allele length and reduction in allele signal, we were also able to estimate the extent of length dependant amplification for the typed markers. Expressing this as the proportionate reduction in peak height per base pair of extra length, we could then use this value as a crude correction for length dependant amplification by inflating the signal for longer alleles accordingly (prior to normalisation). A total of 529 markers were typed in the 16 trio families. These markers were mostly chosen from the extreme (most promising) end of the original UK study, the remainder having given extreme results in other GAMES screens. The mean missing allele proportion was found to be 9.2% and the mean length-dependent amplification factor (proportionate signal reduction per base pair) was 2.9%. Interestingly, this factor was almost double for dinucleotides (3.5%) compared to longer repeats (2.0%), suggesting that length dependent amplification is principally determined by repeat number rather than absolute length. Peaks not corresponding to observed alleles were seen in 72% of markers, 22% having just one aberrant peak, 21% two such peaks and 29% three or more such peaks. As expected, the first peak in the AIP was the most often considered aberrant (corresponding to a stutter band). In order to test the stability of the adapting factors estimated by typing just 16 trio families, we used the data from individual typing of the 10 markers listed in Table 1, each of which had been genotyped in the 745 UK trio family cohort. By considering consecutive 16 family subsets, we were able to generate multiple estimates for the adapting factors and thereby calculate their mean and standard deviation (see Table 3). We were also able to test how often peaks corresponding to relatively common alleles (those with a frequency of >10% in the 745 trio families) are inadvertently excluded when just 16 trio families are typed. No such alleles were excluded when only those strictly absent from the 16 trio family genotyping were considered. However, such alleles were occasionally dropped when their frequency was under estimated such that the estimated

frequency contributed less than a quarter of the corresponding peak height. This was observed once for an allele with a frequency of 16%, once for an allele with a frequency of 11% and twice for an allele with a frequency of 10%. No other high frequency alleles were excluded through adoption of this rule. Thus, it is clear that our policy of excluding peaks when the estimated allele frequency contributes less than one quarter of the peak height results in only a very low probability ( < 0.1%) that data from important alleles will be inadvertently ignored. We feel that the advantage of excluding peaks that are essentially (>75%) stutter bands far outweighs this small risk. 3.4. Refined analysis Employing these adapting factors, we reanalysed all available UK data. This reanalysis was performed both with and without crude correction for length dependant amplification. In each case the di-, tri- and tetra-nucleotides were analysed independently. In the analysis with correction for length dependant amplification, the mean value of 2.9% (per base pair) was applied to all markers not included amongst the 529 specifically typed. In the analysis without correction for length-dependent amplification, we identified a total of 34 markers with empirical p-values < 10% in both screens. Thirty-five such markers were identified in the analysis employing the correction for length-dependent amplification. Results from these two analyses are very similar and substantially overlap, such that in total just 48 extreme markers were identified. This list of extreme markers includes 35 for which no extra AIPs had previously been generated, as well as 28 that were not amongst the 529 for which adapting Table 5 p-values from individual typing of the two new novel markers Marker

Case-controls

Trio families

D5S112 D10S1769

0.1080 0.0956

0.0535 0.0903

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59 Table 6 Ranked position of HLA markers in combined analysis of case-control and family data Marker

Original

Refined

D6S1615 TNFa D6S2444 D6S265 D6S273

31 195 132 95 1015

1 7 11 14 166

factors had been calculated (25 overlapped, that is had neither extra AIPs or appropriate adapting factors). 3.5. Final analysis In order to refine further the new list of most extreme markers, additional replicate AIPs were generated for the 35 markers not previously treated in this way. We also established appropriate adapting factors for the 28 extreme markers not included amongst the 529 previously treated in this manner. As before, these adapting factors were determined by individually genotyping the markers in 16 trio families. A final analysis was performed combining all the available data. In this final analysis, only 5 of these 48 promising markers retained empirical p-values of better than 10% in both case-control and trio family studies (the empirical pvalues for these markers are shown in Table 4a and b). Three markers come from the HLA region (D6S1615, D6S2444 and TNFa). Each is in LD with the class II susceptibility allele DRB1*1501. These are the same three HLA markers identified in our original publication. Results from individual typing of the two novel markers (D5S112

57

and D10S1769) are shown in Table 5. Neither of these novel markers is confirmed as associated. However, there is concordance between the pooled analysis and the results from individual genotyping. The HLA markers listed in Table 4a and b are securely and robustly identified, making it unlikely that other markers showing similar degrees of LD (DV = 52% for TNFa; Coraddu et al., 1998) have been missed. Given that other HLA markers, such as D6S265 and D6S273 (DV = 39% for this marker; Coraddu et al., 1998), show lower degrees of LD with DRB1*1501, it is perhaps not surprising that they are absent from the final list of promising markers. Performing a combined analysis of both the case control and trio family data, in order to compare with the results from our previously published analysis, shows that this refined analysis has improved the ranked position of these less strongly associated markers (see Table 6). This observation suggests that novel markers showing similarly modest degrees of LD (if included in the 6000 considered) are likely also to have moved up the marker ranking—although, with this methodology and sample size, these cannot be disentangled from the background un-associated markers. 3.6. Multipoint analysis In the presence of extensive LD, a genuine susceptibility locus might result in several adjacent markers showing evidence for association. In order to search for such marker clusters, we used the sliding window method suggested by Setakis (2003). In this multipoint analysis, highly significant results were seen in the HLA region on chromosome 6p21 (see Fig. 1). However, no other region of the genome showed evidence for association. This failure is not unex-

Fig. 1. Multipoint p-values along chromosome 6 calculated using a 2cM sliding window: p-values are plotted as log( p). The raw p-values from each marker used in this analysis were those calculated in the combined analysis of data from both the case-control and trio family cohorts.

58

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59

pected. Average LD in the human genome is estimated to be very much less than 100 kb (Gabriel et al., 2002) while the markers employed in this screen have an average separation of some 500 kb. These parameters make it very unlikely that adjacent markers will all be in LD with a local susceptibility locus. The HLA region is a notable exception, in that the marker map employed in this region is denser than the average while the LD is known to be more extensive.

4. Discussion In order to refine the analysis of data generated in our GAMES experiment, we have developed adapting factors for 529 microsatellites that substantially improve the correlation with results obtained from individual genotyping. After individual genotyping, none of the novel (non-HLA) markers identified in the UK screen have been confirmed as associated with multiple sclerosis. Microsatellites from the HLA region, which have high levels of LD with DRB1*1501, are robustly identified confirming that our experiment can detect markers in LD with genuine susceptibility alleles. Conversely, our failure to detect HLA markers with established but lesser degrees of genuine LD (e.g. D6S265 and D6S273) indicates limitations of the study. Three factors limit the study’s power. First is the sample size. This is modest in the UK casecontrol cohort, like most of the other GAMES studies reported in this issue (c200 cases and c200 controls). Even with full and accurate individual genotyping, such cohorts have only modest power to detect effects as strong as TNFa, and little or no power to detect weaker effects such as those for markers like D6S265 and D6S273. This limitation is well illustrated by results obtained after individually typing the available UK case control samples for markers TNFa and D6S1615. These are only just nominally significant, and do not survive correction for multiple testing. It is therefore expected that markers such as TNFa, D6S1615 and D6S2444 will be identified in many but not all GAMES screens. On the other hand markers such as D6S265 and D6S273 may be seen in a few but not the majority of screens. Considering these positive control markers provides great insight into the expected behaviour of these modestly powered screens. The second factor limiting power is our use of pooling methodology. This introduces additional sources of error over and above sampling variance and thereby reduces the effective sample size of the cohorts considered (Barratt et al., 2002). Replicating pool construction, PCR and signal detection improves the signal-to-noise ratio but reduces the efficiency that pooling seeks to achieve. Barratt et al. (2002) have shown that the need for replication increases with pool size. The UK trio family cohort was analysed as two particularly large pools (745 individuals in the index pool and 1490 individuals in the parents pool). Compensating for additional errors in the analysis of these pools would require

significantly more replication than we have performed. At the level of replication employed in the study, the UK trio cohort behaves with an effective sample size similar to that seen for the UK case-control cohort (Sawcer et al., 2002). The difference is that, in the case-control cohort, the dominant source of variance is sampling variance while in the trio family cohort the principal source of variance results from the pooling process (the larger sample size having essentially minimised the effects of sampling variance). This difference in the contribution from the various sources of variance explains why individual genotyping results (Tables 1 and 5) show a better correlation for the case-control than for the trio family cohort. Promising but false positive markers identified in the trio family analysis are more likely to be the result of pooling induced errors than sampling variance, which has a much smaller effect in a sample of this size. The third and final factor determining the power of our study, and each of the other GAMES screens, is the number of markers considered. The figure of 6000 proposed by Barcellos et al. (1997) and utilised in GAMES, derived from the most optimistic estimate of LD available at the time (Jorde, 1995) and was commensurate with the number of markers then available. It is now clear however that LD is far less extensive. Current best estimates suggest that the average length of so-called haplotype blocks is just 22 kb (Gabriel et al., 2002). Assuming that our markers are uniformly distributed (so that no one block includes more than one marker), it is clear that we have markers in no more than 4% of the haplotype blocks making up the human genome. The extent to which a single microsatellite is able to interrogate a block is unknown but it is clear that this is limited by allele frequency mismatch and other confounding effects (Muller-Myhsok and Abel, 1997). Assuming that five tagging SNPs are able to extract most of the information from a block (Johnson et al., 2001), it seems likely that, on average, a single microsatellite will extract no more than 50% of the available information (the greater heterozygosity of a microsatellite making it equivalent to approximately 2.5 SNPs (Kruglyak, 1997)). The greater mutation rate of microsatellites increases the rate at which LD with susceptibility variants is whittled down over time. But, in an expanding and relatively young population such as Caucasians Europeans, this is unlikely to have much effect (Thompson and Neel, 1997). Allowing for these various issues, it seems clear that no more than 1% of the genome will have been tested in a typical GAMES screen (even without allowing for incomplete marker typing). The number of markers employed is clearly the major limiting factor. When comparing results obtained by individual genotyping with the pooled DNA analysis, it is important to remember that not all samples included in the pools were individually genotyped. Some samples used to construct pools were no longer available when individual genotyping was performed (27% for the controls pool) and genotyping was also incomplete (success rate was 92% for D5S112 and

T.W. Yeo et al. / Journal of Neuroimmunology 143 (2003) 53–59

88% for D10S1769). As a result, there is only partial overlap in samples contributing to the pooled and individual typing results. Whilst the overlap is substantial, some difference would be expected even if pooling was perfect and true tests of significance rather than empirical p-values obtained in the analysis. This comparison is further confounded when it is remembered that, although individual genotyping is dogmatically considered as a gold standard, it is not without error. Under the null hypothesis of no associated markers, it would be expected that approximately 60 markers would have p-values of < 10% in both the case-control and trio family cohorts (10% of 10% of 6000). However, in our final analysis, there are only five such markers and three of these are from the HLA region. This marked deficit in the expected number of ‘‘positive in both’’ markers is a result of our recursive concentration on this over-lapping group. In each stage of our experiment, the refining methods have only been applied to these overlapping markers—the most interesting end of the distribution. Since our refining methods are conservative and, designed to reduce variance, they have the tendency to reduce the number of markers falling within the overlapped group. In effect, by concentrating on just the extreme of the distribution, we have improved the specificity of results for markers in this group but have had very little effect on sensitivity of the experiment. Associated markers, where the evidence for association was initially under estimated, will not have been included amongst those for which adapting factors and additional AIPs were generated. Thus, these have a low probability of being included in the final analysis. Although our UK GAMES screen has failed to find any non-HLA markers showing evidence for association, it has enabled us to substantially improve the quality of the analysis and thereby enhanced the power of other GAMES screens.

Acknowledgements We thank Aslaug Jonasdottir, Ragnheidur Fossdal and Jeffrey Gulcher from deCODE for giving us access to their genetic map, which was used in the multipoint analysis of our data. We are grateful to the members of the Association of British Neurologists for notifying patients participating in this study. This study represents one component of the GAMES project designed and coordinated by DASC and SJS

59

(Cambridge, UK) with funding from the Wellcome Trust (grant 057097) supplemented by the Multiple Sclerosis Societies of the United States and Great Britain, and MS International Federation.

References Barcellos, L.F., Klitz, W., Field, L.L., Tobias, R., Bowcock, A.M., Wilson, R., Nelson, M.P., Nagatomi, J., Thomson, G., 1997. Association mapping of disease loci using a pooled DNA genomic screen. American Journal of Human Genetics 61, 734 – 747. Barratt, B.J., Payne, F., Rance, H.E., Nutland, S., Todd, J.A., Clayton, D.G., 2002. Identification of the sources of error in allele frequency estimations from pooled DNA indicates an optimal experimental design. Annals of Human Genetics 66, 393 – 405. Coraddu, F., Sawcer, S., Feakes, R., Chataway, J., Broadley, S., Jones, H.B., Clayton, D., Gray, J., Smith, S., Taylor, C., Goodfellow, P.N., Compston, D.A.S., 1998. HLA typing in the United Kingdom multiple sclerosis genome screen. Neurogenetics 2, 24 – 33. Gabriel, S.B., Schaffner, S.F., Nguyen, H., Moore, J.M., Roy, J., Blumenstiel, B., Higgins, J., DeFelice, M., Lochner, A., Faggart, M., Liu-Cordero, S.N., Rotimi, C., Adeyemo, A., Cooper, R., Ward, R., Lander, E., Daly, M.J., Altshuler, D., 2002. The structure of haplotype blocks in the human genome. Science 296, 2225 – 2229. Johnson, G.C., Esposito, L., Barratt, B.J., Smith, A.N., Heward, J., Di Genova, G., Ueda, H., Cordell, H.J., Eaves, I.A., Dudbridge, F., Twells, R.C., Payne, F., Hughes, W., Nutland, S., Stevens, H., Carr, P., Tuomilehto-Wolf, E., Tuomilehto, J., Gough, S.C., Clayton, D.G., Todd, J.A., 2001. Haplotype tagging for the identification of common disease genes. Nature Genetics 29, 233 – 237. Jorde, L.B., 1995. Linkage disequilibrium as a gene-mapping tool. American Journal of Human Genetics 56, 11 – 14. Kruglyak, L., 1997. The use of a genetic map of biallelic markers in linkage studies. Nature Genetics 17, 21 – 24. Muller-Myhsok, B., Abel, L., 1997. Genetic analysis of complex diseases. Science 275, 1328 – 1329. Poser, C.M., Paty, D.W., Scheinberg, L., McDonald, W.I., Davis, F.A., Ebers, G.C., Johnson, K.P., Sibley, W.A., Silberberg, D.H., Tourtellotte, W.W., 1983. New diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines for research protocols. Annals of Neurology 13, 227 – 231. Sawcer, S., Maranian, M., Setakis, E., Curwen, V., Eva Akesson, E., Hensiek, A., Coraddu, F., Roxburgh, R., Sawcer, D., Gray, J., Deans, J., Goodfellow, P.N., Walker, N., Clayton, D., Compston, A., 2002. A whole genome screen for linkage disequilibrium in multiple sclerosis confirms disease associations with regions previously linked to susceptibility. Brain 125, 1337 – 1347. Setakis, E., 2003. Statistical analysis of the GAMES studies. Journal of Neuroimmunology 143, 47 – 52 (this issue). Thompson, E.A., Neel, J.V., 1997. Alleleic disequilibrium and allele frequency distribution as a function of social and demographic history. American Journal of Human Genetics 60, 197 – 204. Thomson, G., 1995. Mapping disease genes: family-based association studies. American Journal of Human Genetics 57, 487 – 498.