Remote Effects of Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation on Error Processing in Motor Re-Learning

Remote Effects of Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation on Error Processing in Motor Re-Learning

Accepted Manuscript Title: Remote Effects of Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation on Error Processing in Motor Re-Learning Author: Marco Taubert, Thors...

785KB Sizes 0 Downloads 16 Views

Accepted Manuscript Title: Remote Effects of Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation on Error Processing in Motor Re-Learning Author: Marco Taubert, Thorsten Stein, Tommy Kreutzberg, Christian Stockinger, Lukas Hecker, Anne Focke, Patrick Ragert, Arno Villringer, Burkhard Pleger PII: DOI: Reference:

S1935-861X(16)30057-2 http://dx.doi.org/doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.007 BRS 887

To appear in:

Brain Stimulation

Received date: Revised date: Accepted date:

11-11-2015 21-3-2016 10-4-2016

Please cite this article as: Marco Taubert, Thorsten Stein, Tommy Kreutzberg, Christian Stockinger, Lukas Hecker, Anne Focke, Patrick Ragert, Arno Villringer, Burkhard Pleger, Remote Effects of Non-Invasive Cerebellar Stimulation on Error Processing in Motor ReLearning, Brain Stimulation (2016), http://dx.doi.org/doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.007. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1 2 3

Remote effects of non-invasive cerebellar stimulation on error processing in motor re-learning

4 5

Abbreviated title: Error processing in motor re-learning

6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29

Marco Taubert*1, Thorsten Stein*2, Tommy Kreutzberg1, Christian Stockinger2, Lukas Hecker1, Anne Focke2, Patrick Ragert3, Arno Villringer1,4 & Burkhard Pleger5 1

Department of Neurology, Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 04103 Leipzig, Germany 2 YIG "Computational Motor Control and Learning", BioMotion Center, Institute of Sports and Sports Science, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology Karlsruhe, 76131 Karlsruhe, Germany 3 Institute of General Kinesiology and Athletics Training, University of Leipzig, 04109 Leipzig, Germany 4 Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital Leipzig, 04103 Leipzig, Germany 5 Department of Neurology, BG University Hospital Bergmannsheil, Ruhr-University Bochum, 44789 Bochum, Germany *both authors contributed equally to this work

Corresponding author: Dr. Marco Taubert Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences Department of Neurology Stephanstrasse 1a 04103 Leipzig, Germany Phone: +49-341-9940-2216 Fax: +49-341-9940-113 (221) Email: [email protected]

30 31

Conflicts of interest: none

32

Acknowledgements: We thank Sebastian Papke for his help in preparing figure 1.

33 34 35 36

Designed research (MT, TS, AV, BP), performed research (MT, TK, LH), contributed unpublished reagents/analytic tools (TS, CS, AF, PR, BP), analyzed data (MT, TS, TK, CS), wrote the paper (MT, TS, CS, BP).

1 Page 1 of 21

37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44

Highlights   

Sham-controlled, double-blinded tDCS study on the cerebellums role in motor (re)learning A-B-A paradigm employed to create behavioral interference by secondary task (B) and test long-term tDCS effects on motor memory re-acquisition Behavioral interference disrupted motor memory retention but (anodal but not sham or cathodal) tDCS delivered online during memory acquisition induced lasting and robust effects on re-acquisition performance

45 46

ABSTRACT

47

Background: While concurrent transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) affects motor

48

memory acquisition and long-term retention, it is unclear how behavioral interference

49

modulates long-term tDCS effects. Behavioral interference can be introduced through a

50

secondary task learned in-between motor memory acquisition and later recall of the original

51

task.

52

Objective/Hypothesis: The cerebellum is important for the processing of errors if movements

53

should be adapted to external perturbations (motor memory acquisition). We hypothesized

54

that concurrent cerebellar tDCS during adaptation influences both memory acquisition and

55

re-acquisition if motor errors are enlarged due to behavioral interference.

56

Methods: In a sham-controlled and double-blinded study, we applied anodal and cathodal

57

tDCS to the ipsilateral cerebellum while subjects adapted reaching movements to an

58

external, clockwise force field perturbation (acquisition task A) with their dominant right arm.

59

Behavioral interference by an oppositely oriented, counter-clockwise perturbation (secondary

60

task B) was introduced in between the acquisition and re-acquisition (24h later) sessions.

61

Results: Learning task B disrupted memory retention of A and re-increased motor errors in

62

the re-acquisition session. Anodal but not sham or cathodal tDCS impaired motor memory

63

acquisition and, additionally, increased motor errors during re-acquisition of the original

64

motor memory.

2 Page 2 of 21

65

Conclusion(s): Behavioral interference disrupted motor memory retention but tDCS delivered

66

online during memory acquisition induced lasting and robust effects on re-acquisition

67

performance one day later. Our data also suggests different error-processing mechanisms at

68

work during motor memory acquisition and re-acquisition.

69

Keywords: transcranial direct current stimulation, cerebellum, force field adaptation,

70

interference, anodal, cathodal

71

INTRODUCTION

72

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive brain stimulation technique to

73

modulate brain function and behavior [1]. If applied concurrently during motor training, tDCS

74

has the potential to influence learning and memory [2, 3]. In particular, tDCS facilitates or

75

inhibits the neurophysiological processes underlying memory acquisition, consolidation and

76

retention [3-5].

77

Error-based motor learning offers the possibility to investigate mechanisms of motor memory

78

acquisition and interference [6]. One typical task example is arm reaching movements that

79

are perturbed by a robotic device. Over time, participants learn to compensate for these

80

perturbations [7]. In the time after successful adaptation to a given force field A (acquisition),

81

the associated motor memory is susceptible to degradation through adaptation to a new,

82

interfering force field B that perturbs movements in the opposite direction. When the original

83

force field is revisited one day later (re-acquisition, A-B-A paradigm), the errors that occur

84

during re-learning may be comparable to the initial learning session depending on the time

85

interval between the adaptation to force field A and B [8, 9].

86

The cerebellum is strongly involved in error-based motor learning [10-13]. Patients with acute

87

lesions or degeneration of the cerebellar cortex have difficulties to adapt reaching

88

movements to external (force field) perturbations [14-16]. Particularly impaired is the initial

89

adaptation to large perturbation-induced motor errors which is conceptualized as fast

90

learning in the multiple-state model of motor learning [17-19]. Following this functional 3 Page 3 of 21

91

implication, we hypothesized a cerebellar involvement not only during initial adaptation to

92

force field A, but also during re-adaptation to A if motor errors are enlarged due to an

93

interfering force field B.

94

In the present sham-controlled and double-blinded study, we applied tDCS to the cerebellum

95

while healthy participants adapted reaching movements to a force field A. TDCS is widely

96

used to non-invasively alter activity of the cerebellum or other cortical regions [20, 21]

97

allowing inference on the causal behavioral role of the stimulated region [3]. After adaptation

98

to force field A, participants were subjected to a secondary reaching task without tDCS,

99

perturbed by an oppositely oriented force field B [9]. The adaptation to force field A was

100

revisited one day later, but this time without simultaneous tDCS.

101

We hypothesized that tDCS specifically influences the fast error-processing during motor

102

memory acquisition of A. Subsequent interference through learning of B will force subjects to

103

recapitulate initial motor errors that ultimately unfold a remote tDCS-induced memory-deficit

104

during re-acquisition of A.

105

4 Page 4 of 21

106

METHODS

107

Participants

108

We recruited 43 right-handed participants (age 27 ± 3 years; 15 female, 28 male). The study

109

was performed in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local

110

ethics committee of the University of Leipzig. All participants were naïve to the experimental

111

paradigm and underwent a neurological examination before participation. Handedness was

112

verified using Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [22]. Participants were randomly assigned to

113

three groups receiving either anodal, cathodal or sham tDCS. Data from two participants of

114

the cathodal tDCS group was lost because of technical problems with the storage device.

115

The study groups were composed as follows: sham=15, anodal=14 and cathodal cerebellar

116

tDCS=12 participants (age 27 ± 3 years; 13 female, 28 male; Table 1).

117

Robotic manipulandum (“BioMotionBot”)

118

The task procedure is similar to Focke et al. [23] and Stockinger et al. [24] and explained in

119

detail in the supplemental materials. Briefly, the “BioMotionBot” applied forces [25] while

120

participants reached to one of eight targets around a center position within a horizontal

121

plane. To avoid sequence effects, the target sequence differed for each participant.

122

Movement sets consisted of 16 trials – eight outwards and eight inwards movements – in

123

which each peripheral target point occurred once. Participants were requested to reach the

124

target within 500 ± 50 ms (movement time). A green circle appeared around the target if

125

movement time was 500 ± 50 ms. Red and orange circles appeared if subjects moved too

126

slow or too fast. Visual feedback was provided throughout the experiment to ensure constant

127

movement time. The “BioMotionBot” generated a clockwise (A) and counterclockwise (B)

128

velocity-dependent force field that applied forces (

129

movement direction.

130

tDCS

) perpendicular to the

5 Page 5 of 21

131

The experimenter performing force field training (TK or LH) was blinded and unaware of the

132

type of tDCS application until the end of the experiment. Another experimenter (MT) attached

133

the tDCS electrodes and monitored the stimulation. TDCS was applied on day 1 during

134

adaptation to force field A with a pair of surface-soaked sponge electrodes (5 × 5 cm) using a

135

commercial tDCS device (NeuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany). A constant current of 2 mA

136

(current density 0.08 mA/cm2) was applied to the right cerebellar hemisphere over a period of

137

20 min. In the anodal stimulation condition, the anode was placed 2 cm below the inion and 1

138

cm posterior to the right mastoid process [26] (see Fig. 1A). The cathode was placed over

139

the right musculus buccinator [21] for an almost right-angled orientation of the current in

140

relation to the cerebellar surface. For cathodal stimulation, anode and cathode were placed

141

contrariwise. For sham tDCS, the constant current of 2 mA was applied, according to

142

common practice, for only 30 s before being switched off [27]. TDCS was turned on 30

143

seconds prior to A1 and covered on average ¾ of the entire learning period (approx. 25-30

144

minutes).

145

Experimental design

146

We used an ABA-paradigm [8] to investigate motor memory interference (Fig. 1B). On day 1,

147

we first familiarized participants under null field conditions (25 sets, 400 trials) ensuring

148

reaching movements in 500 ± 50 ms. After 5 min of rest, a baseline block (96 trials) was

149

conducted under null field conditions and this data was used to exclude between-group

150

differences prior to tDCS. After another 5 min of rest, 25 sets (400 trials) were performed in

151

force field A (A1) with 60 seconds breaks after each five sets (set breaks). 2.5 hours later,

152

participants were exposed to force field B (B = -A) for 25 sets (400 trials). After 24 h on day

153

2, participants performed another 25 sets (400 trials) in force field A (A2).

154

Confounding effects of sleep, physical activity and caffeine on memory consolidation were

155

controlled by interviewing participants on both days (Table 1).

6 Page 6 of 21

156

During A1, B and A2, set-breaks of 60 s were inserted after each five sets (80 trials) and

157

participants could release their hand from the handle but remained seated. This allowed us

158

to test tDCS-effects on fast forgetting [17, 18]. Each force field session lasted for

159

approximately 30 min. Participants were instructed to sleep at least 6h between day 1 and 2.

160

Data Analysis

161

Reaching error

162

Data preprocessing was performed using the custom-made software “ManipAnalysis” ([28];

163

see supplemental material). To quantify the reaching error for each trial, we calculated the

164

perpendicular displacement of the hand trajectory in centimeter from a straight line joining

165

start and target point 300 ms after movement start (PD300) similar to previous studies [24, 29,

166

30].

167

Statistics

168

Reaching errors in the baseline block (sets 1-6; Fig. 1B) were averaged and compared

169

between groups to exclude differences in baseline behavior (one-way ANOVA).

170

Following Jayaram et al. [31], performance errors in each condition (A1, B and A2) were

171

averaged for two practice phases: (1) early practice phase when performance error is large

172

(sets 1-5) as well as (2) late practice phase when motor performance reached plateau levels

173

(sets 21-25).

174

First, (behavioral) memory interference was tested using reaching errors in early and late

175

phases of A1, B and A2 under sham tDCS (repeated measures (RM) ANOVA with within-

176

subject factors FORCE FIELD (A1, B, A2) and PRACTICE PHASE (early phase, late

177

phase)). Because reaching errors in B were in the opposite direction of A (B=-A), we

178

calculated the modulus of reaching errors in B.

179

Next, tDCS effects in the early and late learning phases were evaluated with RM-ANOVA

180

with within-subject factors PRACTICE PHASE (early phase, late phase), FORCE FIELD (A1, 7 Page 7 of 21

181

A2) and between-subject factor STIMULATION (anodal, cathodal, sham tDCS). We

182

hypothesized tDCS effects to be specific to A1 and A2 and tested for potential carry-over

183

effects of tDCS in a separate RM-ANOVA of B (within-subject factor PRACTICE PHASE

184

(early phase, late phase) and between-subject factor STIMULATION (anodal, cathodal,

185

sham tDCS). Moreover, we performed correlation analyses to assess how performance at

186

the end of A1 and B related to the starting performance in A2. Therefore, mean reaching

187

errors occurring in the late phase of A1 and B (sets 21-25) and the early phase of A2 (sets 1-

188

5) were correlated.

189

In line with the multiple-state model of motor learning [19], we assumed that tDCS-

190

modulation of A1 and A2 is characterized by a double dissociation between fast, rapidly

191

decaying (A1) as well as slow, persistent memory components (A2). Therefore, we also

192

analyzed absolute retention [32] (mean value of sets 6,11,16,21 after corresponding set

193

break) across set breaks and refer to this parameter as set-break forgetting [17, 18]. TDCS-

194

induced changes in set-break forgetting were evaluated with RM-ANOVA (within-subject

195

factors BLOCK (sets after set breaks: 6, 11, 16, 21), FORCE FIELD (A1, A2) and between-

196

subject factor STIMULATION (anodal, cathodal, sham tDCS).

197

All analyses were Greenhouse-Geisser corrected if sphericity was violated. Main and

198

interaction effects were considered significant if p < 0.05. Multiple comparison correction of

199

post-hoc two-sided t-tests was performed according to Bonferroni. We specified effect sizes

200

of ANOVAs using partial eta squared η2p (small effect: η2p ≥ 0.01; medium effect η2p ≥ 0.06;

201

large effect: η2p ≥ 0.14).

8 Page 8 of 21

202

RESULTS

203

All groups were comparable in age, gender, physical activity level and sleep duration (Table

204

1). Baseline performance (null field) did not significantly differ between groups (F(2,38)=0.803;

205

p=0.456, η2p=0.041).

206

Memory interference

207

In line with our previous studies [23, 24], we found a reduction of mean reaching errors from

208

the early to the late practice phases in A1, B and A2 under sham tDCS (main effects of

209

PRACTICE PHASE (F(1,14)=502.879; p<0.001, η2p=0.973) and FORCE FIELD

210

(F(2,28)=36.231; p<0.001, η2p=0.721) and PRACTICE PHASE x FORCE FIELD interaction

211

(F(2,28)=18.041; p<0.001, η2p=0.563)) with larger errors occurring in the early phase of B

212

(p<0.001 for B vs. A1 and B vs. A2), and larger mean reaching errors under A2 compared to

213

A1 (p=0.009 for A1 vs. A2). This latter result indicates that the intended interference effect

214

was achieved by the A-B-A design.

215

Effect of tDCS on acquisition (A1) and re-acquisition (A2)

216

Analysis of mean reaching errors

217

Analysis of mean reaching errors during early and late phases in both force fields revealed

218

significant main effects of STIMULATION (F(2,38)=3.824, p=0.031, η2p=0.17), PRACTICE

219

PHASE (F(1,38)=1111.702, p<0.001, η2p=0.967) and FORCE FIELD (F(1,38)=42.019, p<0.001,

220

η2p=0.525). Additionally, tDCS interacted with PRACTICE PHASE (F(2,38)=4.013, p=0.026,

221

η2p=0.174) and FORCE FIELD (F(2,38)=3.725, p=0.033, η2p=0.164) and a triple-interaction

222

emerged (F(2,38)=3.781, p=0.032, η2p=0.166). Subsequent ANOVA’s for each practice phase

223

(early, late) indicated tDCS-modulation in the early but not late phase (early: FORCE FIELD

224

(F(1,38)=41.092, p<0.001, η2p=0.52), STIMULATION (F(2,38)=4.648, p=0.016, η2p=0.197) and

225

FORCE FIELDxSTIMULATION interaction (F(2,38)=4.877, p=0.013, η2p=0.204); late: FORCE

226

FIELD (F(1,38)=7.869, p=0.008, η2p=0.172), STIMULATION (F(2,38)=2.481, p=0.097,

9 Page 9 of 21

227

η2p=0.115) and FORCE FIELDxSTIMULATION interaction (F(2,38)=0.113, p=0.894,

228

η2p=0.006)).

229

More specifically, Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc t-tests (p<0.0083) for the early phase

230

indicated that anodal tDCS impaired reaching performance during re-acquisition (A2:

231

p=0.0034 anodal vs. sham, Fig. 2) but not acquisition (A1). For the late phase A1 and A2, no

232

significant differences emerged (passing p<0.0083, see supplement). These results reveal

233

no significant differences between the three groups in the late phase of A1. However, we

234

found a significant difference between the sham and anodal tDCS group in the early phase of

235

A2, indicating that behavioral effects of anodal tDCS are still visible after 24 hours during

236

motor memory re-acquisition.

237

Additionally, we conducted correlation analyses to assess how performance at the end of A1

238

and B related to the starting performance in A2. Under sham tDCS, mean errors in the late

239

phase of A1 positively correlated with mean errors in the early phase of A2 (R2=0.504,

240

p=0.003, two-sided; Fig. 3). However, errors in the late phase of B were not correlated with

241

errors in the early phase of A2 (R2=0.069, p=0.343), suggesting that the larger the errors at

242

the end of A1, the larger the errors at the beginning of A2, but not B. The positive correlation

243

(sham tDCS) between performance in the late phase of A1 and the early phase of A2 was

244

absent under anodal (R2=0.065, p=0.380) and cathodal tDCS (R2=0.292, p=0.069). Thus,

245

despite behavioral interference through B, memory re-acquisition (A2) was affected by tDCS,

246

applied one day earlier.

247

Analysis of set-break forgetting

248

RM-ANOVA of set-break forgetting revealed main effects of FORCE FIELD (F(1,38)=5.637,

249

p=0.023, η2p=0.129), BLOCK (F(3,114)=132.231, p<0.001, η2p=0.777) and STIMULATION

250

interacted with FORCE FIELD (F(2,38)=3.614, p=0.037, η2p=0.160). Subsequent ANOVA’s of

251

each block [within-subject factor: force field (A1, A2); between-subject factor: stimulation

252

(anodal, cathodal, sham tDCS)] revealed only for the first block a FORCE FIELD x 10 Page 10 of 21

253

STIMULATION interaction (F(2,38)=6.538, p=0.004, η2p=0.256) indicating different effects of

254

the stimulation on set break forgetting in A1 and A2 (see supplement for ANOVAs of each

255

block as well as further correlation analysis). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc t-tests

256

(p<0.0083) for the first block indicate that anodal tDCS increased set-break forgetting during

257

acquisition (A1: p=0.0057 anodal vs. sham, p=0.0035 anodal vs. cathodal, p=0.392 cathodal

258

vs. sham) but not re-acquisition (A2: p=0.033 anodal vs. sham, p=0.725 anodal vs. cathodal,

259

p=0.045 cathodal vs. sham; Figs. 4 and 5). In summary, these results suggest that anodal

260

tDCS modulated the fast-learning-fast-forgetting component during memory acquisition [19].

261

Confounding influence by attention

262

To control for unspecific tDCS-induced cognitive changes in attention (Ferrucci et al., 2008),

263

we tested for tDCS-effects on reaction times between subsequent reaching movements (time

264

from appearance of a new target until leaving the current target zone) and found no

265

significant tDCS influences (see supplement).

266

Effect of tDCS on acquisition of B (day 1)

267

Finally, we tested the force-field selectivity of our effects and analyzed the adaptation to force

268

field B. Note that tDCS was only applied during learning of A1 and 2.5h between A1 and B

269

were chosen to prevent any tDCS after effects on B. RM-ANOVA revealed significant

270

reduction of mean reaching errors (main effect of PRACTICE PHASE (F(1,38)=866.000,

271

p<0.001, η2p=0.958)) but no main effect of STIMULATION (F(2,38)=0.380, p=0.687, η2p=0.020)

272

and no significant interaction between PRACTICE PHASE and STIMULATION (F(2,38)=0.978,

273

p=0.386, η2p=0.049). Also, set-break forgetting was not influenced by tDCS indicated by

274

absent main effects of STIMULATION (F(2,38)=0.306, p=0.738, η2p=0.016) and BLOCK x

275

STIMULATION interaction (F(3,114)=1.590, p=0.156, η2p=0.077; Fig. 6). However, we found a

276

significant main effect of BLOCK (F(3,114)=141.967, p<0.001, η2p=0.789). These results are in

277

line with our hypothesis that there is no effect of tDCS on reaching errors in B.

278 11 Page 11 of 21

279

DISCUSSION

280

We show that cerebellar anodal tDCS delivered during the adaptation to perturbed reaching

281

movements caused deficits in learning-related error processing and hence short-term

282

memory retention between practice blocks, whereas the subsequently learned interference

283

task (i.e., perturbations by a counter-rotated force field) remained unaffected, indicating a

284

task-specific effect of tDCS. The key findings of our study are that cerebellar tDCS induced

285

(1) impairments in short-term memory retention during initial acquisition of task A and (2)

286

stable performance deficits in the early phase of the re-acquisition session 24 hours later.

287

This indicates robust and remote tDCS effects possibly due to an erroneous memory

288

formation or/and an erroneous information transfer between consecutive motor memory

289

states [6, 33-35].

290

Effects of cerebellar tDCS on motor learning

291

The present data suggests differences between cerebellar error-processing during motor

292

memory acquisition and re-acquisition induced by anodal tDCS. Cathodal tDCS instead

293

showed no significant effects, but a vague trend into the same inhibitory direction as for

294

anodal tDCS (see Fig. 2). These polarity-unspecific effects are consistent with a number of

295

previous cerebellar tDCS studies [26, 36-39]. Both anodal and cathodal cerebellar tDCS

296

have been shown to either facilitate or impair motor learning or cognitive functioning [21, 26,

297

31, 36-43]. Based on the inhibitory effects shown by the majority of cerebellar tDCS studies,

298

independently on whether tDCS was applied with anodal or cathodal polarity, Ferrucci and

299

Priori [1] recently hypothesized that cerebellar tDCS may generally interfere with the

300

inhibitory nature of Purkinje cell long-term depression (LTD) by altering the fine-tuning of

301

membrane potential and the relative pace-making properties. This hypothesis clearly

302

contradicts many other cerebellar tDCS studies [31, 44, 45] that showed a facilitative effect

303

especially when tDCS was applied with anodal polarity and during task performance.

304

Herzfeld et al. [45] was the only study using force field adaptation and cerebellar tDCS. In

12 Page 12 of 21

305

contrast to our study, they found an impairment of error-based learning through cathodal

306

cerebellar tDCS and improvements through anodal tDCS.

307

We used the same tDCS protocol [21, 26, 27] as Herzfeld and colleagues and the samples of

308

both studies are generally comparable in age and gender distribution. However, a major

309

difference between both studies is that Herzfeld and colleagues analyzed the effects of tDCS

310

on the acquisition and retention of a force field A (“savings” analyzed with an “A-A design”).

311

In contrast, the purpose of our study was to analyze how tDCS affects the acquisition and

312

retention of a force field A within a behavioral interference design (“consolidation” with an “A-

313

B-A design”) [61]. These differences most likely affect the retention on day 2, but cannot

314

explain the differing results between the two studies in the acquisition of force field A on day

315

1. However, there are a couple of differences in the behavioral tasks and practice schedules

316

of both studies: Herzfeld and colleagues used a different amount and direction of reaching

317

targets (16 targets in our study [center-in and center-out movements] vs. 2 targets in [45]), a

318

different number of force field trials (400 in our study vs. 199 in [45]), as well as different

319

force field strengths (k=20 Ns/m in our study and k=13 Ns/m in [45]). Furthermore, the

320

practice schedule in our study consisted five blocks each with 80 field trials and four breaks

321

of 60 s in length without any practice. In contrast, the practice schedule of Herzfeld and

322

colleagues consisted of ten blocks with 21 field trials with 3 randomly inserted error-clamp

323

trials, followed by 30 error-clamp trials. Followed by an eleventh block with 24 field trials

324

including five error-clamp trials. Previous studies in force field learning revealed that the

325

practice schedule [24, 50, 51] and the perturbation protocol [14, 23] affect the acquisition and

326

retention of motor memories. Therefore, these differences in the experimental design of both

327

studies may explain the conflicting results, since cerebellar involvement during error-based

328

learning is highly task-dependent [11, 46]. Consequently, we speculate that the interaction

329

between learning-induced neuroplasticity and cerebellar tDCS [47, 48] may explain the

330

discrepant findings between our and previous studies [45]. In support of this, Holmström et

331

al. found that cerebellar activity was modulated by both the exerted forces as well as the

332

instability of the controlled objects [49]. Furthermore, the practice schedule [24, 50, 51] and 13 Page 13 of 21

333

the perturbation protocol [14] have been shown to affect motor adaptation and consolidation.

334

We speculate that these factors may have interacted with the neural structures and their

335

associated plasticity during practice [14, 42, 52], leading to distinct behavioral outcomes [53].

336

Thus, the two experimental designs different in our study in many aspects, suggesting that

337

not only the polarity of tDCS itself, but rather the interaction between tDCS and the specific

338

task characteristics decide on whether the effect is facilitative or inhibitory. Accordingly,

339

future studies should consider different movement tasks and practice schedules as

340

independent variables to gain a deeper understanding of how tDCS affects the acquisition

341

and retention in force field learning.

342

Fast and slow mechanisms of motor adaptation

343

It has been suggested that regions in the cerebellar cortex are involved in fast learning while

344

primary motor cortex or cerebellar nuclei mediate slow learning processes [17, 42, 54]. This

345

view is consistent with evidence from associative learning studies indicating initial

346

performance impairments through reversible inactivation of the cerebellar cortex while focal

347

lesions in the cerebellar interpositus nucleus specifically impair long-term memory retention

348

[55]. In line with these findings, patients with cerebellar cortical degeneration show difficulties

349

to adapt their motor commands to large force related perturbations of reaching movements

350

(abrupt perturbations), whereas learning from small errors (gradual perturbations) is

351

preserved [14]. These results together with other studies [10, 15, 31, 46, 52] suggest a

352

cerebellar role in fast and slow learning-induced motor error reduction [56]. Following this

353

functional implication, we assumed that the cerebellum is not only involved in error reduction

354

during motor memory acquisition, but also during later stages when interference through a

355

secondary task affords memory re-acquisition. In line with the proposed fast and slow time

356

scales of motor adaptation [19, 34] and recent evidence for error memory [35], cerebellar

357

tDCS during initial memory acquisition induced impairments in error-processing specifically

358

during set breaks. In the early phase of the relearning session one day later, tDCS effects

359

translated into deficits in mean reaching errors and set-break impairments were no longer 14 Page 14 of 21

360

visible. These findings suggest that anodal tDCS affected not only the cerebellar cortex but

361

also the deeper located cerebellar nuclei or the interconnected primary motor cortex with

362

profound influences on both, fast learning processes in the initial learning session and slow

363

learning processes in the relearning session 24 hours later.

364

The key finding of our study is that behavioral effects of cerebellar tDCS are still visible after

365

24 hours during the phase of motor memory re-acquisition. Based on the literature [20], it is

366

unlikely that these behavioral effects are mediated by lingering physiological changes

367

induced by 20 minutes of tDCS applied one day earlier. In contrast to the tDCS effects on

368

A1, we found no changes in set-break forgetting but a more generally impaired error

369

processing across movement sets in the early phase of the relearning session (see Fig. 5).

370

Although, in the sham tDCS condition motor errors in early and late learning phases were

371

comparable during memory acquisition (A1) and re-acquisition (A2) (Fig. 2), set-break

372

forgetting strongly differed between A1 and A2. Thus, while motor errors were on average

373

comparable between A1 and A2, the difference in retention rates is consistent with the view

374

that different error-processing mechanisms are at work during initial learning and relearning

375

[34]. TDCS seems to specifically interact with this memory transfer suggesting a long-lasting,

376

robust but functionally dynamic effect of online cerebellar tDCS on motor memories. This

377

view is supported by previous behavioral [33, 35, 57] and functional brain imaging studies

378

[58, 59]. Criscimagna-Hemminger and Shadmehr [57] found spontaneous recovery of an

379

original force field memory that was believed to be blocked through acquisition of a

380

competing memory. The authors concluded that a competing learning task does not produce

381

unlearning of the beforehand learned task, but rather installs a new competing but fragile

382

memory [57]. Our results support this claim and suggest that memory components acquired

383

during A1 are masked but not erased by B. This is also consistent with recent observations

384

of a specific error memory that is formed during initial memory acquisition and recognized

385

during its re-acquisition [35]. Re-testing in our study may re-engage these memory

386

components and unfold a long-term behavioral tDCS effect.

15 Page 15 of 21

387

Limitation

388

We did not assess the effects of tDCS on error processing without the interference task B to

389

disentangle the influence of tDCS on A2 from effects introduced by the interfering task.

390

However, the primary goal of this study was to prove that the effects of tDCS are still visible

391

upon retesting 24 hours later, although a competing task was learned in-between.

392

Disentangling the effects of tDCS from those induced by the interference task itself is the

393

next consequent step for future studies.

394

Conclusion

395

Combining tDCS with a motor interference task, we extend previous findings of long-term

396

tDCS effects on motor learning [4, 60] and provide evidence that tDCS induced robust and

397

specific effects on motor memory retention despite prior behavioral interference. TDCS-

398

induced behavioral impairments suggest differences between cerebellar error processing

399

during initial motor memory acquisition and its interference-induced re-acquisition. We

400

conclude that a short session of tDCS may not only influence concurrent motor learning but

401

also long-term performance in the presence of a strong behavioral interference.

402 403

References

404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420

1. Ferrucci R, Priori A. Transcranial cerebellar direct current stimulation (tcDCS): motor control, cognition, learning and emotions. Neuroimage. 2014;85 Pt 3:918-23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.122. PubMed PMID: 23664951. 2. Orban de Xivry JJ, Shadmehr R. Electrifying the motor engram: effects of tDCS on motor learning and control. Exp Brain Res. 2014;232(11):3379-95. doi: 10.1007/s00221-0144087-6. PubMed PMID: 25200178; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4199902. 3. Dayan E, Censor N, Buch ER, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(7):838-44. doi: 10.1038/nn.3422. PubMed PMID: 23799477. 4. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(5):1590-5. Epub 2009/01/24. doi: 0805413106 [pii] 10.1073/pnas.0805413106. PubMed PMID: 19164589; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2635787. 5. Ziemann U, Siebner HR. Modifying motor learning through gating and hemostatic metaplasticity. Brain Stimulation. 2008;1(1):60-6. 16 Page 16 of 21

421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475

6. Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:89-108. doi: 10.1146/annurevneuro-060909-153135. PubMed PMID: 20367317. 7. Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci. 1994;14(5 Pt 2):3208-24. PubMed PMID: 8182467. 8. Robertson EM, Pascual-Leone A, Miall RC. Current concepts in procedural consolidation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(7):576-82. doi: 10.1038/nrn1426. PubMed PMID: 15208699. 9. Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzi E. Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature. 1996;382(6588):252-5. Epub 1996/07/18. doi: 10.1038/382252a0. PubMed PMID: 8717039. 10. Tseng YW, Diedrichsen J, Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R, Bastian AJ. Sensory prediction errors drive cerebellum-dependent adaptation of reaching. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98(1):54-62. doi: 10.1152/jn.00266.2007. PubMed PMID: 17507504. 11. Diedrichsen J, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Dissociating timing and coordination as functions of the cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2007;27(23):6291-301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0061-07.2007. PubMed PMID: 17554003; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2216743. 12. Izawa J, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Cerebellar contributions to reach adaptation and learning sensory consequences of action. J Neurosci. 2012;32(12):4230-9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6353-11.2012. PubMed PMID: 22442085; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3326584. 13. Shmuelof L, Krakauer JW. Are we ready for a natural history of motor learning? Neuron. 2011;72(3):469-76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.017. PubMed PMID: 22078506; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3389513. 14. Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Bastian AJ, Shadmehr R. Size of error affects cerebellar contributions to motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(4):2275-84. doi: 10.1152/jn.00822.2009. PubMed PMID: 20164398; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2853280. 15. Donchin O, Rabe K, Diedrichsen J, Lally N, Schoch B, Gizewski ER, et al. Cerebellar regions involved in adaptation to force field and visuomotor perturbation. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107(1):134-47. doi: 10.1152/jn.00007.2011. PubMed PMID: 21975446. 16. Burciu RG, Reinold J, Rabe K, Wondzinski E, Siebler M, Muller O, et al. Structural correlates of motor adaptation deficits in patients with acute focal lesions of the cerebellum. Exp Brain Res. 2014;232(9):2847-57. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3956-3. PubMed PMID: 24798401. 17. Xu-Wilson M, Chen-Harris H, Zee DS, Shadmehr R. Cerebellar contributions to adaptive control of saccades in humans. J Neurosci. 2009;29(41):12930-9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3115-09.2009. PubMed PMID: 19828807; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2994243. 18. Ethier V, Zee DS, Shadmehr R. Spontaneous recovery of motor memory during saccade adaptation. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99(5):2577-83. doi: 10.1152/jn.00015.2008. PubMed PMID: 18353917; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2733835. 19. Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. Interacting adaptive processes with different timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol. 2006;4(6):e179. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040179. PubMed PMID: 16700627; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1463025. 20. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 2000;527 Pt 3:633-9. PubMed PMID: 10990547. 21. Galea JM, Jayaram G, Ajagbe L, Celnik P. Modulation of cerebellar excitability by polarity-specific noninvasive direct current stimulation. J Neurosci. 2009;29(28):9115-22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2184-09.2009. PubMed PMID: 19605648; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2760225. 22. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97-113. PubMed PMID: 5146491. 17 Page 17 of 21

476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529

23. Focke A, Stockinger C, Diepold C, Taubert M, Stein T. The influence of catch trials on the consolidation of motor memory in force field adaptation tasks. Frontiers in psychology. 2013;4:479. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00479. PubMed PMID: 23898319; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3722502. 24. Stockinger C, Focke A, Stein T. Catch trials in force field learning influence adaptation and consolidation of human motor memory. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:231. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00231. PubMed PMID: 24795598; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4001009. 25. Bartenbach V, Sander C, Poschl M, Wilging K, Nelius T, Doll F, et al. The BioMotionBot: a robotic device for applications in human motor learning and rehabilitation. J Neurosci Methods. 2013;213(2):282-97. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2012.12.006. PubMed PMID: 23276545. 26. Ferrucci R, Marceglia S, Vergari M, Cogiamanian F, Mrakic-Sposta S, Mameli F, et al. Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation impairs the practice-dependent proficiency increase in working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20(9):1687-97. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20112. PubMed PMID: 18345990. 27. Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117(4):845-50. PubMed PMID: 16427357. 28. Stockinger C. PM, Focke A., Stein T. ManipAnalysis - a software application for the analysis of force field experiments. Int J Comput Sci Sport. 2012;(11):52–7. 29. Shadmehr R, Brashers-Krug T. Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor memory. J Neurosci. 1997;17(1):409-19. PubMed PMID: 8987766. 30. Donchin O, Sawaki L, Madupu G, Cohen LG, Shadmehr R. Mechanisms influencing acquisition and recall of motor memories. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(4):2114-23. PubMed PMID: 12364533. 31. Jayaram G, Tang B, Pallegadda R, Vasudevan EV, Celnik P, Bastian A. Modulating locomotor adaptation with cerebellar stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 2012;107(11):2950-7. doi: 10.1152/jn.00645.2011. PubMed PMID: 22378177; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3378372. 32. Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1999. 33. Pekny SE, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Protection and expression of human motor memories. J Neurosci. 2011;31(39):13829-39. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1704-11.2011. PubMed PMID: 21957245; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3208234. 34. Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA. Biological Learning and Control How the Brain Builds Representations, Predicts Events, and Makes Decisions. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 2012. 35. Herzfeld DJ, Vaswani PA, Marko MK, Shadmehr R. A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning. Science. 2014;345(6202):1349-53. doi: 10.1126/science.1253138. PubMed PMID: 25123484; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4506639. 36. Panouilleres MT, Miall RC, Jenkinson N. The role of the posterior cerebellum in saccadic adaptation: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. J Neurosci. 2015;35(14):5471-9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4064-14.2015. PubMed PMID: 25855165; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4388915. 37. Panouilleres MT, Joundi RA, Brittain JS, Jenkinson N. Reversing motor adaptation deficits in the ageing brain using non-invasive stimulation. J Physiol. 2015. doi: 10.1113/JP270484. PubMed PMID: 25929230. 38. Macher K, Bohringer A, Villringer A, Pleger B. Cerebellar-parietal connections underpin phonological storage. J Neurosci. 2014;34(14):5029-37. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0106-14.2014. PubMed PMID: 24695720. 39. Grimaldi G, Argyropoulos GP, Boehringer A, Celnik P, Edwards MJ, Ferrucci R, et al. Non-invasive cerebellar stimulation--a consensus paper. Cerebellum. 2014;13(1):121-38. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0514-7. PubMed PMID: 23943521. 18 Page 18 of 21

530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585

40. Boehringer A, Macher K, Dukart J, Villringer A, Pleger B. Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation modulates verbal working memory. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(4):649-53. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.001. PubMed PMID: 23122917. 41. Shah B, Nguyen TT, Madhavan S. Polarity independent effects of cerebellar tDCS on short term ankle visuomotor learning. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(6):966-8. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.04.008. PubMed PMID: 23711765. 42. Orban de Xivry JJ, Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Contributions of the motor cortex to adaptive control of reaching depend on the perturbation schedule. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(7):1475-84. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq192. PubMed PMID: 21131448; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3116732. 43. Cantarero G, Spampinato D, Reis J, Ajagbe L, Thompson T, Kulkarni K, et al. Cerebellar direct current stimulation enhances on-line motor skill acquisition through an effect on accuracy. J Neurosci. 2015;35(7):3285-90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.288514.2015. PubMed PMID: 25698763; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4331640. 44. Galea JM, Vazquez A, Pasricha N, de Xivry JJ, Celnik P. Dissociating the roles of the cerebellum and motor cortex during adaptive learning: the motor cortex retains what the cerebellum learns. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(8):1761-70. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq246. PubMed PMID: 21139077; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3138512. 45. Herzfeld DJ, Pastor D, Haith AM, Rossetti Y, Shadmehr R, O'Shea J. Contributions of the cerebellum and the motor cortex to acquisition and retention of motor memories. Neuroimage. 2014;98:147-58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.076. PubMed PMID: 24816533; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4099269. 46. Diedrichsen J, Hashambhoy Y, Rane T, Shadmehr R. Neural correlates of reach errors. J Neurosci. 2005;25(43):9919-31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-05.2005. PubMed PMID: 16251440; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC1479774. 47. Sami S, Miall RC. Graph network analysis of immediate motor-learning induced changes in resting state BOLD. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:166. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00166. PubMed PMID: 23720616; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3654214. 48. Sami S, Robertson EM, Miall RC. The time course of task-specific memory consolidation effects in resting state networks. J Neurosci. 2014;34(11):3982-92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4341-13.2014. PubMed PMID: 24623776; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3951697. 49. Holmstrom L, de Manzano O, Vollmer B, Forsman L, Valero-Cuevas FJ, Ullen F, et al. Dissociation of brain areas associated with force production and stabilization during manipulation of unstable objects. Exp Brain Res. 2011;215(3-4):359-67. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2903-9. PubMed PMID: 22038714; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3950331. 50. Caithness G, Osu R, Bays P, Chase H, Klassen J, Kawato M, et al. Failure to consolidate the consolidation theory of learning for sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J Neurosci. 2004;24(40):8662-71. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2214-04.2004. PubMed PMID: 15470131. 51. Overduin SA, Richardson AG, Lane CE, Bizzi E, Press DZ. Intermittent practice facilitates stable motor memories. J Neurosci. 2006;26(46):11888-92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1320-06.2006. PubMed PMID: 17108162. 52. Schlerf JE, Galea JM, Bastian AJ, Celnik PA. Dynamic modulation of cerebellar excitability for abrupt, but not gradual, visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci. 2012;32(34):11610-7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1609-12.2012. PubMed PMID: 22915105; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3435878. 53. Jones KT, Berryhill ME. Parietal contributions to visual working memory depend on task difficulty. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:81. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00081. PubMed PMID: 22973241; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3437464. 54. Medina JF, Garcia KS, Mauk MD. A mechanism for savings in the cerebellum. J Neurosci. 2001;21(11):4081-9. PubMed PMID: 11356896. 55. Thompson RF. In search of memory traces. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005;56:1-23. Epub 2005/02/16. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070239. PubMed PMID: 15709927. 19 Page 19 of 21

586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602

56. Dayan E, Cohen LG. Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron. 2011;72(3):443-54. Epub 2011/11/15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008. PubMed PMID: 22078504; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3217208. 57. Criscimagna-Hemminger SE, Shadmehr R. Consolidation patterns of human motor memory. J Neurosci. 2008;28(39):9610-8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3071-08.2008. PubMed PMID: 18815247; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2665258. 58. Shadmehr R, Holcomb HH. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. Science. 1997;277(5327):821-5. PubMed PMID: 9242612. 59. Imamizu H, Miyauchi S, Tamada T, Sasaki Y, Takino R, Putz B, et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool. Nature. 2000;403(6766):192-5. Epub 2000/01/26. doi: 10.1038/35003194. PubMed PMID: 10646603. 60. Kaminski E, Hoff M, Sehm B, Taubert M, Conde V, Steele CJ, et al. Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) during complex whole body motor skill learning. Neurosci Lett. 2013;552:76-80. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.07.034. PubMed PMID: 23933205. 61. Krakauer JW, Shadmehr R. Consolidation of motor memory. Trends in Neurosciences. 2006;29:58-64.

603

Figure 1: Cerebellar tDCS and experimental design. (A) Anodal tDCS was applied over the right cerebellar

604

hemisphere (anode) with the cathode placed over the right buccinator muscle. For cathodal tDCS the electrodes

605

were positioned contrariwise. For sham (i.e., placebo) tDCS, the constant current of 2 mA was applied for only 30

606

sec before being switched off. (B) All participants of the three groups performed reaching movements in a

607

classical ABA interference paradigm with familiarization and baseline trials under null field conditions prior to A on

608

day 1. Participants received either anodal, cathodal or sham tDCS over the right cerebellar hemisphere during

609

adaptation to force field A on day 1 (A1). Note that tDCS was not applied during the interference task (force field

610

B) on day 1 or during relearning of force field A on day 2 (A2). Short breaks of 60 seconds were interspersed after

611

each of five sets in A1, B and A2.

612

Figure 2: Mean reaching errors (perpendicular displacement at 300 ms) in the early and late phases of

613

adaptation to A (A1) and relearning A (A2) for anodal, cathodal and sham tDCS. Error bars show SEM. Asterisks

614

indicate significant differences (Bonferroni corrected for multiple comparisons) between tDCS conditions while

615

crosses indicate statistical trends (p < 0.1).

616

Figure 3: Correlations between mean errors at the end of A1 and the beginning of A2 for sham (black), anodal

617

(red) and cathodal (green) tDCS. Note that a positive correlation was observed for sham tDCS but not anodal or

618

cathodal tDCS.

619

Figure 4: Performance in force field A (A1). Reaching errors for each set under anodal, cathodal and sham tDCS

620

during initial adaptation to A (A1). Error bars show SEM. Asterisk indicates significant differences (Bonferroni

621

corrected for multiple comparisons) and cross indicates statistical trend between tDCS conditions.

20 Page 20 of 21

622

Figure 5: Reaching errors for each set under anodal, cathodal and sham tDCS during relearning A (A2). Anodal

623

tDCS significantly increased reaching errors in the early practice phase of A2 while set break forgetting was

624

preserved (see text for statistical analyses). Error bars show SEM.

625

Figure 6: Reaching errors for each set under anodal, cathodal and sham tDCS during interference learning in

626

force field B. Error bars show SEM. No significant differences were found between tDCS conditions.

627 628

Tab. 1: Sample characteristics and statistical comparisons using univariate ANOVA

anodal tDCS

cathodal tDCS

sham tDCS

(n = 14)

(n = 12)

(n = 15)

value)

26.14 (2.51)

27.83 (3.64)

27.27 (2.60)

0.326

4

4

5

2.36 (0.63)

2.58 (0.79)

2.47 (0.83)

0.752

2.57 (1.16)

2.75 (1.06)

2.13 (0.99)

0.307

body-mass index

23.29 (2.23)

26.50 (7.42)

24.07 (4.03)

0.233

sleep duration

7.29 (0.85)

7.50 (1.19)

7.03 (1.43)

0.598

7.57 (0.65)

7.21 (1.08)

8.03 (0.88)

0.060

age (M ± SD) gender (# females) physical activity

statistics (p-

-

(days per week) physical activity (hours per week)

(day 1) sleep duration (day 2) 629

21 Page 21 of 21